Adolphs, R
(2015) The unsolved problems of neuroscience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(4), 173–175. Elsevier Ltd. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Albertazzi, L
(1973) Immanent realism. An introduction to Brentano. Philosophia Mathematica, Vol. 1–10. Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Alexander, Samuel
(1920) Space, time and deity, Vol. 2. London: MacMillan.Google Scholar
Amedi, A., Merabet, L. B., Camprodon, J., Bermpohl, F., Fox, S., Ronen, I., Kim, D.-S., & Pascual-Leone, A
(2008) Neural and behavioral correlates of drawing in an early blind painter: A case study. Brain Research, 1242(November), 252–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andrews, T. J
(2001) Binocular rivalry and visual awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(10), 407–409. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Antonova, C., & Kemp, M
(2010) Space, time, and presence in the Icon. Seeing the World with the Eyes of God. Ashgate: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Armstrong, D
(1961) Perception and the physical world. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
(1968) A materialist theory of mind. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Aru, J., Bachmann, T., Singer, W., & Melloni, L
(2012) Distilling the neural correlates of consciousness. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(2), 737–746. Elsevier Ltd. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Austin, J. L
(1962) Sense and Sensibilia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ayer, A. J
(1940) The foundations of empirical knowledge. London: MacMillan.Google Scholar
(1967) Has Austin refuted the sense-datum theory? Synthese, 17, 117–140. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bagley, M.-O., & Maxfield, M. S
(1986) Afterimage color perception for designers. Perception & Motor Skills, 63(2), 995–1007. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S
(2004) The chronoarchitecture of the human brain – Natural viewing conditions reveal a time-based anatomy of the brain. NeuroImage, 22, 419–433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bennett, M. R., Dennett, D. C., Hacker, P., & Searle, J. R
(2007) Neuroscience and philosophy: Brain, mind, and language. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Bergmann, G
(1960) Duration and the specious present. Philosophy of Science, 27(1), 39–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergson, H
(1950) Time and free will. New York: McMillan.Google Scholar
Bidwell, S
(1896) On subjective colour phenomena attending sudden changes in illumination. Proceedings of The Royal Society B, 60, 368–377. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1897) On negative after-images following brief retinal extinction. Proceedings of The Royal Society B, 61, 268–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Billock, V. A., Gleason, G. A., & Tsou, B. H
(2001) Perception of forbidden colors in retinally stabilized equiluminant images: An indication of softwired cortical color opponency? Journal of Optical Society of America, 18, 2398–2403. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Billock, V. A., & Tsou, B. H
(2010) Seeing forbidden colors. Scientific American, 302(2), 58–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Block, N
(2005) Review of Alva Noë’s ‘Action in Perception.’ The Journal of Philosophy, 102(5), 259–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(5–6), 481–499.Google Scholar
Bonjour, L
(2004) In search of direct realism. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, LXIX(2), 349–368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brecht, B
(1945) Life of Galileo.Google Scholar
Brentano, F
(1874) Psychology from an empirical standpoint. London: Rout.Google Scholar
Brewer, B
(2006a) Perception and content. European Journal of Philosophy, 1999, 165–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006b) Perception and its objects. Philosophical Studies, 132(1), 87–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) How to account for illusion. In A. Haddock & F. Macpherson (Eds.), Disjunctivism: Perception, action and knowledge (pp. 169–180). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2011) Perception and its objects. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brindley, G. S
(1963) Afterimages. Scientific American, 209(4), 84–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Broad, C. D
(1925) The mind and its place in nature. London: Routledge Kegan & Paul, p. 674.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A
(1990) Elephants don’t play chess. Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 6, 3–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1991) Intelligence without representation. Artificial Intelligence, 47, 139–159. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, J. L
(1965) Afterimages. In C. H. Graham (Ed.), Vision and visual perception (pp. 479–503). Philadelphia: Saunders.Google Scholar
Brugger, P
(2008) The Phantom Limb in Dreams. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(4), 1272–1280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brugger, P., Kollias, S. S., Müri, R. M., Crelier, G., Hepp-Reymond, M.-C. C., & Regard, M
(2000) Beyond re-membering: Phantom sensations of congenitally absent limbs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(11), 6167–6172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruntrup, G., & Jaskolla, L. J
(2016) Panpsychism: Contemporary perspectives. Edited by G. Bruntrup & L. J. Jaskolla. New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burge, T
(1979) Individualism and the mental. Book Section. Edited by French, Uehling, and Wettstein. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
(2003) Social anti-individualism, objective reference. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 67(3), 682–690. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Byrne, A., & Hilbert, D. R
(2003) Color realism and color science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 3–64.Google Scholar
Byrne, A., & Logue, H
(2009) Disjunctivism. Contemporary readings. Cambridge (Mass): The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Callender, C
(2002) Time, reality, and experience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cameron, R. P
(2006) Tropes, necessary connections, and non-transferability. Dialectica, 60(2), 99–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Campbell, J
(2002) Reference and consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cargile, T
(2003) On ‘Alexander’s’ Dictum. Topoi, 22, 143–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carroll, N
(2008) The philosophy of motion pictures. Malden (Mass): Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P
(2006) The architecture of the mind. New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) The opacity of mind. An integrative theory of self-knowledge. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casali, A. G., Casarotto, S., Rosanova, M., Mariotti, M., & Massimini, M
(2010) General indices to characterize the electrical response of the cerebral cortex to TMS. NeuroImage, 49(2), 1459–1468. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, D. J
(1995) Facing up to the problem of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2(3), 200–219.Google Scholar
(1996) The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2003) The Matrix as Metaphysics. Matrix Website, 1–26.Google Scholar
(2008) Foreword. In A. Clark (Ed.), Supersizing the mind (pp. 1–33). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2014) How do you explain consciousness? Ted Talks, March.Google Scholar
(2015) Panpsychism and Panprotopsychism. In T. Alter & Y. Nagasawa (Eds.), Consciousness in the physical world: Perspectives on Russellian monism (pp. 246–276). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Changeux, J.-P. P
(2004) Clarifying consciousness. Nature, 428, 603–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chemero, A
(2009) Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chisholm, R. M
(1986) Brentano and intrinsic value. Cambridge (Mass): Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M
(2005) Chimerical colors: Some phenomenological predictions from cognitive neuroscience. Philosophical Psychology, 18(5), 527–560. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cicogna, P. C., Occhionero, M., Natale, V., & Esposito, M. J
(2007) Bizarreness of size and shape in dream images. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 381–390. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, A
(2008) Supersizing the mind. Book, 8(6), 79–99. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. J
(1998) The extended mind. Analysis, 58(1), 10–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J
(2009) The red and the real. An essay on color ontology. New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coleman, S
(2013) The real combination problem: Panpsychism, micro-subjects, and emergence. Erkenntnis, January.Google Scholar
Cook, M
(1996) Descartes and the dustbin of the mind. History of Philosophy Quarterly, 13(1), 17–33.Google Scholar
Craik, K. J. W
(1940) Origin of visual after-images. Nature, 145, 512. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1966a) On the effect of looking at the sun. In S. L. Sherwood (Ed.), The nature of psychology (pp. 98–101). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(1966b) The nature of psychology. A selection of papers, essays, and other writings by Kenneth J. W. Craik. Edited by S. J. Sherwood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Crane, H. D., & Piantanida, T. P
(1983) On seeing reddish green and yellowish blue. Science, 221(4615): 1078–1080. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crary, J
(1988) Techniques of the observer. October, 45(3), 3–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crick, F
(1994) The astonishing hypothesis: The scientific search for the soul. New York: Touchstone.Google Scholar
Crick, F., & Koch, C
(1990) Toward a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Seminars in Neuroscience, 2, 263–295.Google Scholar
(1998) Consciousness and neuroscience. Cerebral Cortex, 8(2), 92–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davies, P
(2005) About time. Einstein’s unfinished revolution. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Davis, G., & Driver, J
(1994) Parallel detection of Kanisza subjective figures in the human visual system. Nature, 371, 791–792. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daw, N. W
(1967) Goldfish retina: Organization for simultaneous color contrast author. Science, 158(3803), 942–944. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daxecker, F
(1992) Christoph Scheiner’s eye studies. Documenta Ophthalmologica, 81(1), 27–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Valois, R. L
(1965) Analysis and coding of color vision in the primate visual system. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology, 30, 567–579. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Valois, R. L., & De Valois, K. K
(1993) A multi-stage color model. Vision Research, 33(8), 1053–1065. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S
(2014) Consciousness and the brain: Deciphering how the brain codes our thoughts. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J.-P. P
(2011) Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron, 70(2), 200–227. Elsevier Inc. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C
(1978) Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology, 1st ed. Montgomery: Bradford Books.Google Scholar
(1987) The intentional stance. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1991) Consciousness explained, 1st ed. Boston: Little Brown and Co.Google Scholar
(1996) Kinds of minds: Toward an understanding of consciousness, 1st ed., Vol. 4. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Dewey, J
(1916) Democracy and education: An introduction to the philosophy of education. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Diekelmann, S., & Born, J
(2010) The memory function of sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 114–126.Google Scholar
Dolev, Y
(2007) Time and realism: Metaphysical and antimetaphysical perspectives, Vol. 69. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dowe, P
(1992) Wesley Salmon’s process theory of causality and the conserved quantity theory. Philosophy of Science, 59, 195–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995) What’s right and what’s wrong with transference theories. Erkenntnis, 42, 363–374. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000) Physical causation. New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drake, S
(1957) Discoveries and opinions of Galileo. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Dretske, F. I
(1996) Phenomenal externalism, or if meanings ain’t in the head, Where Are Qualia? Philosophical Issues, 7(1996), 143–158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J
(2008) The obscure act of perception. Philosophical Studies, 139(3), 367–393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eddington, A. S
(1929) The nature of the physical world. New York: MacMillan.Google Scholar
Edelman, G. M
(1989) The remembered present: A biological theory of consciousness. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
(2001) Consciousness: The remembered present. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 929, 111–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Einstein, A
(1916) Relativity. London: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elder, C. L
(2003) Alexander’s dictum and the reality of familiar objects. Topoi, 22, 163–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farkas, K
(2003) What is externalism? Philosophical Studies, 112(3), 187–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feyerabend, P. K
(1958) Reichenbach’s interpretation of quantum mechanics. Philosophical Studies, 9, 45–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ffytche, D. H
(2005) Visual hallucinations: Charles Bonnet syndrome. Current Psychiatry Reports, 7(December), 168–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ffytche, D. H., Howard, R. J., Brammer, M. J., David, A., Woodruff, P., & Williams, S
(1998) The anatomy of conscious vision an fMRI study of visual hallucinatios. Nature Neuroscience, 1(8), 738–792. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Field, G. D., Gauthier, J. L., Sher, A., Greschner, M., Machado, T. A., Jepson, L. H., Shlens, J., Gunning, D. E., Mathieson, K., Dabrowski, W., Paninski, L., Litke, A. M., & Chichilnisky, E. J
(2010) Functional connectivity in the retina at the resolution of photoreceptors. Nature, 467, 673–678. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Freud, S
(1920) A general introduction to psychoanalysis. New York: Bartelby. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frey, M., & von Kries, J
(1881) Ueber Die Mischung von Spectralfarben. Archiv Fur Anatomie Und Physiologie, Physiologische Abtheilun, 336–353.Google Scholar
Gage, J
(1999) Colour and meaning. London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
Gaines, L. S., & Vetter, H. J
(1968) Sensory deprivation and psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice, 5(1), 7–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gale, R. M
(1971) Has the present any duration? Noûs, 5(1), 39–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Galilei, G
(1623) The Assayer. In Discoveries and opinions of Galileo, Translated by S. Drake. Garden City, NY: Masterworks Program.Google Scholar
Geisler, W. S
(1978) Adaptation, afterimages and cone saturation. Vision Research, 18, 279–289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gendler, T. S., & Hawthorne, J
(2004) Conceivability and possibility. Edited by T. S. Gendler & J. Hawthorne. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Goff, P
(2009) Can the panpsychist get around the combination problem? In D. Skrbina (Ed.), Mind that Abides. Panpsychism in the New Millenium (pp. 129–146). Antwerp: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gold, K., & Rabins, P. V
(1989) Isolated visual hallucinations and the Charles Bonnet syndrome: A review of the literature and presentation of six cases. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 30(1), 90–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, B. E
(2010) Sensation and perception. Australia: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Goller, A. I., Richards, K., Novak, S., & Ward, J
(2013) Mirror-touch synaesthesia in the Phantom Limbs of Amputees. Cortex, 49(1), 243–251. Elsevier Srl. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A
(2013) Mindless. The new Neuro-Skeptiks. The New Yorker, September, 7–13.Google Scholar
Gordon, J. E
(1991) Theories of visual perception. Optometry and vision science, 68(9), 758. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grandin, T
(1996) Thinking in pictures: Other reports from My Life with Autism. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Haddock, A., & Macpherson, F
(2008) Disjunctivism: Perception, action, knowledge. Edited by A. Haddock & F. Macpherson. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haggard, P
(2009) Neuroscience. The sources of human volition. Science, 324(5928), 731–733. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Decision time for free will. Neuron, 69(3), 404–406. Elsevier Inc. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Halliwell, J. J., Perez-Mercander, J., & Zurek, W. H
(1994) Physical origins of time asymmetry. Edited by J. J. Halliwell, J. Perez-Mercander, & W. H. Zurek. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harman, G
(1990) The intrinsic quality of experience. Philosophical Perspectives, 4(1990), 31–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harnad, S
(1990) The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42, 335–346. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994) Computation is just interpretable symbol manipulation; cognition isn’t. Minds and Machines, 4(4), 379–390. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harnad, S., & Scherzer, P
(2003) Can a machine be conscious? How? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 83, 435–451.Google Scholar
(2008) First, scale up to the robotic turing test, then worry about feeling. Artificial Intelligence in Medicine, 44, 83–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haynes, J.-D., & Rees, G
(2006) Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 523–534. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haynes, J.-D., Sakai, K., Rees, G., Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Passingham, R. E
(2007) Reading hidden intentions in the human brain. Current Biology, 17(4), 323–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hedges, T. R
(2007) Charles Bonnet, his life, and his syndrome. Survey of Ophthalmology, 52(1), 111–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hering, E
(1878) Zur Lehre Vom Lichtsinne. Wien: C. Gerold’s Son.Google Scholar
(1964) Outline of a theory of light sense. Edited by L. M. Hurvich. Harvard: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hobson, A. J
(2002) Dreaming. An introduction to the science of sleep. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hobson, A. J., Sangsanguan, S., Arantes, H., & Kahnl, D
(2011) Dream logic – The inferential reasoning paradigm. Dreaming, 21(1), 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hobson, K
(2011) In defense of relational direct realism. European Journal of Philosophy, 21(41), 550–574. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoefer, C
(1996) The metaphysics of space-time substantivalism. The Journal of Philosophy, 93(1), 5–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hofer, H., Carroll, J., Neitz, J., Neitz, M., & Williams, D. R
(2005) Organization of the human trichromatic cone mosaic. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(42), 9669–9679. Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York 14627-0270, USA. [email protected]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hofer, H., Singer, B., & Williams, D. R
(2005) Different sensations from cones with the same photopigment. Journal of Vision, 5(5), 444–454. Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA. [email protected]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holcombe, A. O
(2009) Seeing slow and seeing fast: Two limits on perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(5), 216–221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holman, E. L
(2008) Panpsychism, physicalism, neutral monism and the Russellian theory of mind. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15(2), 48–67.Google Scholar
Hopkins, R
(2000) Touching pictures. The British Journal of Aesthetics, 40(1), 149–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horovitz, S. G., Braun, A. R., Carr, W. S., Picchioni, D., Balkin, T. J., Fukunaga, M., & Duyn, J. H
(2009) Decoupling of the brain’s default mode network during deep sleep. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(27), 11376–11381. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsieh, P.-J., & Tse, P. U
(2006) Illusory color mixing upon perceptual fading and filling-in does not result in ‘Forbidden Colors.’ Vision Research, 46(14), 2251–2258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N
(2004) Brain and visual perception: The story of a 25-year collaboration. New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hudson, H
(2003) Alexander’ S Dicta and Merricks’ Dictum. Topoi, 22, 173–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hume, D
(1758) An enquiry concerning human understanding. Chicago: Gateway.Google Scholar
Hurley, S. L
(1998) Vehicles, contents, conceptual structure, and externalism. Analysis, 58(1), 1–6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hurley, Susan L
(2003) Action, the unity of consciousness, and vehicle externalism. Book Section. Edited by Axel Cleeremans. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hurley, S. L
(2010) The varieties of externalism. In R. Menary (Ed.), The Extended Mind (pp. 101–155). Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hurovitz, C. S., Dunn, S., Domhoff, W. G., & Fiss, H
(1999) The dreams of blind men and women: A replication and extension of previous findings. Dreaming, 9(2/3), 183–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hurvich, L. M
(1981) Color vision. Cambridge (Mass): Sinauer Associates Inc.Google Scholar
Hurvich, L. M., & Jameson, D
(1957) An opponent-process theory of color vision. Psychological Review, 64, Part 1(6), 384–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E
(2016) Evolving enactivism: Basic minds meet content. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackson, F., & Pargetter, R
(1977) Relative simultaneity in the special relativity. Philosophy of Science, 44(3), 464–474. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacquette, D
(2004) The Cambridge companion to Brentano. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
James, W
(1890) The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt and Company. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jameson, D., & Hurvich, L. M
(1961) Opponent chromatic induction: Experimental evaluation and theoretical account. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 51(1), 46–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnston, M
(2002) Appearance and reality. In Manifest. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
(2004) The obscure object of hallucination. Philosophical Studies, 120, 113–183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Objective mind and the objectivity of our minds. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 75(2), 233–268. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jones, O. R
(1972) After-images. American Philosophical Quarterly, 9(2), 150–158.Google Scholar
Kammer, T., Puls, K., Strasburger, H., Hill, N. J., & Wichmann, F. A
(2005) Transcranial magnetic stimulation in the visual system. I. The psychophysics of visual suppression. Experimental Brain Research, 160(1), 118–128. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kanai, R., Paulus, W., & Walsh, V
(2010) Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation (tACS) modulates cortical excitability as assessed by TMS-induced phosphene thresholds. Clinical Neurophysiology, 121(9), 1551–1554. International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kanai, R., & Tsuchiya, N
(2012) Qualia. Current Biology, 22(10), R392–R396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kanizsa, G
(1985) Seeing and thinking. Acta Psychologica, 59, 23–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kean, S
(2014) The tale of the duelling neurosurgeons. New York: Little, Brown and Company.Google Scholar
Kelly, D. H., & Martinez-Uriegas, E
(1993) Measurements of chromatic and achromatic afterimages. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 10(1), 29–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, J. M
(1980) Pictures and the blind. Journal of the University Film Association, 32(1), 11–22.Google Scholar
(1993) Drawing and the blind. London: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
(2003) Drawings from Gaia, a blind girl. Perception, 32(3). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, J. M., & Juricevic, I
(2006) Esref Armagan and perspective in tactile pictures. Report.Google Scholar
Kerr, N. H., & Domhoff, W. G
(2004) Do the blind literally ‘see’ in their dreams? A critique of a recent claim that they do. Dreaming, 14(4), 230–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kiefer, M., & Pulvermüller, F
(2012) Conceptual representations in mind and brain: Theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions. Cortex, 48(7), 805–825. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kim, J
(1993a) Mental causation in a physical world. Philosophical Issues, 3(1993), 157–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1993b) The non-reductivist’s troubles with mental causation. In J. Heil & A. R. Mele (Eds.), Mental Causation (pp. 189–210). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
(1995) Mental causation: What? me worry ? Philosophical Issues, 6(1995), 123–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1998) Mind in a physical world. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2005) Physicalism, or something near enough. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Kirchhoff, M. D
(2012) Enaction: Toward a new paradigm for cognitive science. Philosophical Psychology, 26(1), 163–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koch, C
(2004) The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Englewood (Col): Roberts & Company Publishers.Google Scholar
Koch, C., Massimini, M., Boly, M., & Tononi, G
(2016a) Neural correlates of consciousness: Progress and problems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(5), 307–321. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016b) The neural correlates of consciousness: Progress and problems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience in press, 5, 307–321. Nature Publishing Group. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koenig, D., & Hofer, H
(2011) The absolute threshold of cone vision. Journal of Vision, 11(1), 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krueger, J. W
(2010) Doing things with music. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 10(1), 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. S
(1962) The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kunzendorf, R. G., Hartmann, E., Cohen, R., & Cutler, J
(1997) Bizarreness of the dreams and daydreams reported by individuals with thin and thick boundaries. Dreaming, 7(4), 265–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kupers, R., Pietrini, P., Ricciardi, E., & Ptito, M
(2011) The nature of consciousness in the visually deprived brain. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(February), 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lacroix, R., Melzack, R., Smith, D., & Mitchell, N
(1992) Multiple phantom limbs in a child. Cortex, 28(3), 503–507. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lamme, V. A. F
(2003) Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 12–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lamme, V. A F
(2006) Towards a true neural stance on consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(11), 494–501. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lamme, V. A. F
(2006) Zap! Magnetic tricks on conscious and unconscious vision. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 193–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lang, H
(1987) Color vision theories in nineteenth century Germany between idealism and empiricism. Color Research and Application, 12, 270–281. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Le Morvan, P
(2004) Arguments against direct realism and how to counter them. American Philosophical Quarterly, 41(3), 221–234.Google Scholar
Lehar, S
(2003) Gestalt isomorphism and the primacy of subjective conscious experience: A Gestalt Bubble Model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(4), 375–444. Mahwah (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leibniz, G. W
(1704) Nouveaux Essais Sur L’entendement humain/New Essays on Human Understanding. Edited by P. Remnant & J. Bennett. Cambridge (Mass): Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Levine, J
(1998) Conceivability and the metaphysics of mind. Noûs, 32(4), 449–480. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewis, D. K
(1986) Philosophical papers, Vol. II. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Libet, B
(1993) Neurophysiology of consciousness: Selected papers and new essays by Benjamin Libet. Boston: Birkhauser. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004) Mind time. The temporal factor in consciousness. Cambridge (Mass): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lindberg, D. C
(1976) Theories of vision from Al-Kindi to Kepler. Chicago: University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Livingstone, M. S
(2002) Vision and art: The biology of seeing. New York, NY: Harry N. Abrams.Google Scholar
Livitz, G., Yazdanbakhsh, A., Eskew, R. T., & Mingolla, E
(2011) Perceiving opponents hues in color induction displays. Seeing and Perceiving, 24(1), 1–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Llewellyn, S
(2013) Such stuff as dreams are made on? Elaborative encoding, the ancient art of memory, and the hippocampus. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 589–607. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Llinàs, R
(2001) I of the vortex: From neurons to self. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lopes, D. M
(1996) Understanding pictures. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(1997) Art media and the sense modalities: Tactile pictures. The Philosophical Quarterly, 47(189), 425–440. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) Vision, touch, and the value of pictures. The British Journal of Aesthetics, 42(2), 191–201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lycan, W. G
(2002) The case for phenomenal externalism. Book Section. Edited by James E Tomberlin. Noûs, 35, 17–36, Atascadero: Ridgeview Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mach, E
(1897) The analysis of sensations. New York: Dover Publications. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macpherson, F
(2013) The philosophy and psychology of hallucination: An introduction. In F. Macpherson & D. Platchias (Eds.), Hallucination. Philosophy and Psychology (pp. 1–38). Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macpherson, F., & Platchias, D
(2013) Hallucination. Philosophy and psychology. Edited by F. Macpherson & D. Platchias. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahowald, M. W
(1997) Synchrony, sleep, dreams, and consciousness: Clues from K-Complexes. Neurology, 49, 909–911. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Majeed, R
(2013) A representationalist argument against contemporary panpsychism. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5, 105–123.Google Scholar
Manzotti, R
(2006) Brentano ’ S Immanent Realism and Beyond. Mind and Matter, 9, 1–6.Google Scholar
(2008) Does process externalism support panpsychism? In D. Skrbina (Ed.), Mind That Abides. Panpsychism in the New Millenium, 1, (pp. 201–220). Netherlands: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) When and where is information? APA Newsletter on Philosophy and Computers, 10(2), 14–21.Google Scholar
Manzotti, R., & Moderato, P
(2010) Is neuroscience the forthcoming ‘Mindscience’? Behaviour and Philosophy, 38(August 2009), 1–28.Google Scholar
(2013) Neuroscience: Dualism in disguise. In A. Lavazza & H. Robinson (Eds.), Contemporary Dualism (pp. 81–97). New York: Routldege.Google Scholar
Manzotti, R
(2016) A perception-based model of complementary afterimages. SAGE Open, 10(October-November), 1–10.Google Scholar
Maquet, P
(2001) The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science, 294, 1048–1052. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marr, D
(1982) Vision. S.Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Marr, D., & Poggio, T
(1979) A computational theory of human stereo vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 204, 301–328.Google Scholar
Martin, M. G. F
(2002) The transparency of experience. Mind and Language, 17(4), 376–425. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004) Uncovering appearances. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marx, G
(1959) Groucho and me. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
McDowell, J
(1994) Mind and world. Cambridge (Mass): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
McGeoch, P. D., & Ramachandran, V. S
(2012) The appearance of new phantom fingers post-amputation in a phocomelus. Neurocase, 18(2), 95–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Melzack, R
(1992) Phantom limbs. Scientific American, 266, 120–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001) Pain and the neuromatrix in the brain. Journal of Dental Education, 65(12), 1378–1382.Google Scholar
Melzack, R., Israel, R., Lacroix, R., & Schultz, G
(1997) Phantom limbs in people with congenital limb deficiency or amputation in early childhood. Brain, 120(September), 1603–1620. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Merricks, T
(2001) Objects and persons. Oxford: Oxford Clarendon Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meyer, K
(2012) Another remembered present. Science, 335(6067), 415–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A
(1983) Informavores. In F. M. Machlup Una (Ed.), The study of information: Interdisciplinary messages (pp. 111–113). New York: Wiley-Interscience.Google Scholar
Miller, G
(2005) What is the biological basis of consciousness? Science, 309, 79.Google Scholar
Millikan, R. G
(1993) Content and vehicle. In N. Eilan, R. McCarthy, & B. Brewer (Ed.), Spatial representation (pp. 256–268). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Montero, B. G
(1999) The body problem. Noûs, 33(2), 183–200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Must physicalism imply the supervenience of the mental on the physical? The Journal of Philosophy, 5, 93–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muybridge, E
(1893) Descriptive Zoopraxography, or the Science of Animal Locomotion Made Popular. Chicago World's Columbian Exposition. Zoopraxographical Hall.Google Scholar
Mørch, H. H
(2014) Panpsychism and causation. A new argument and a solution to the combination problem. PhD Thesis.Google Scholar
Müller, J
(1840) Handbuch Der Physiologie. Coblenz: Verlag von J. Holscher.Google Scholar
Myers, G. E
(1957) Perception and the ‘time-lag’ argument. Analysis, 17(5), 97–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagel, T
(1974) What is it like to be a bat? The Philosophical Review, 4(4), 435–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noë, A
(2004) Action in perception. Cambridge (Mass): The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2009) Out of our heads. Why you are not your brain, and other lessons from the biology of consciousness. New York: Hill and Wang.Google Scholar
Noë, A., & Thompson, E
(2002) Vision and mind: Selected readings in the philosophy of perception. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2004) Are there neural correlates of consciousness? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11(1), 3–28.Google Scholar
O’Regan, K. J
(1992) Solving the real mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46(3), 461–488. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Why red doesn’t sound like a bell. Understanding the feel of consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Regan, K. J., & Noë, A
(2001a) What it is like to see: A sensorimotor theory of perceptual experience. Synthese, 129, 79–103. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001b) A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 939–973. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Padgham, C. A
(1968) Measurement of the colour sequences in positive visual after-images. Vision Research, 8, 939–949. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Palmer, S. E
(1999a) Vision science. Photons to phenomenology. Cambridge (Mass): MIT AI-Lab.Google Scholar
(1999b) Color, consciousness, and the isomorphism constraint. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(6), 923–943. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peacock, K. A
(1992) A new look at simultaneity. PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association, 1992(1), 542–552. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Penfield, W., & Rasmussen, T
(1950) The cerebral cortex of man. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Penfield, W
(1972) The electrode, the brain and the mind. Zeitschrift Für Neurologie, 201(4), 297–309.Google Scholar
Penfield, W., & Perot, P
(1963) The brain’s record of auditory and visual experience: A final summary and discussion. Brain, 86(4), 595–696. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Penfield, W., & Rasmussen, T
(1950) The cerebral cortex of man. A clinical study of localization of function. New York: MacMillan Company.Google Scholar
Pfeifer, R., & Bongard, J
(2006) How the body shapes the way we think: A new view of intelligence (Bradford Books). New York: Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Pietrini, P., Furey, M. L., Ricciardi, E., Gobbini, I. M., Wu, W.-H. C., Cohen, L. G., Guazzelli, M., & Haxby, J. V
(2004) Beyond sensory images: Object-based representation in the human ventral pathway. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(15), 5658–5663. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S
(1997) How the mind works. USA: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Place, U. T
(1956) Is consciousness a brain process? The British Journal of Psychology, 47, 44–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1988) Thirty years on – Is consciousness still a brain process? Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 2, 208–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pockett, S
(2003) How long is ‘now’? Phenomenology and the specious present. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 2, 55–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Power, S. E
(2010) Perceiving external things and the time-lag argument. European Journal of Philosophy, 21(1), 94–117.Google Scholar
(2011) The metaphysics of the ‘specious’ present. Erkenntnis, 77(1), 121–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pridmore, R. W
(2008) Chromatic induction: Opponent color or complementary color process? Color Research and Application, 33(1), 77–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Complementary colors theory of color vision: Physiology, color mixture, color constancy and color perception. Color Research and Application, 36(6), 394–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prinz, J. J
(2005) Is consciousness embodied? In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition (pp. 1–20). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Proulx, M. J
(2011) Consciousness: What, how, and why. Science, 332(6033), 1034–1035. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ptito, M., Fumal, A., de Noordhout, A. M., Schoenen, J., Albert, G., & Kupers, R
(2008) TMS of the occipital cortex induces tactile sensations in the fingers of blind braille readers. Experimental Brain Research, 184(2), 193–200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Purves, D., & Lotto, R. B
(2002) The empirical basis of color perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 11(4), 609–629. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Putnam, H
(1975) Mind, language and reality. Edited by Hilary Putnam. Vol. 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W
(1984) Computation and cognition: Towards a foundation for cognitive science. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S
(1998) Consciousness and body image: Lessons from phantom limbs, Capgras syndrome and pain asymbolia. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 353, 1851–1859. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., & McGeoch, P. D
(2008) Phantom penises in transsexuals: Evidence of an innate gender-specific body image in the brain. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15(1), 3–26.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., & Rogers-Ramachandran, D
(2000) Phantom limbs and neural plasticity. Archives of Neurology, 57(3), 317–320. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rao, A., Nobre, A. C., Alexander, I., & Cowey, A
(2007) Auditory evoked visual awareness following sudden ocular blindness: An EEG and TMS investigation. Experimental Brain Research, 176, 288–298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rees, G., Kreiman, G., & Koch, C
(2002) Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(4), 261–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reichenbach, H
(1958) The philosophy of space and time. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
(1971) The direction of time. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Revonsuo, A
(2000) Prospects for a scientific research programme on consciousness. Edited by Thomas Metzinger. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2006) Inner presence. Consciousness as a biological phenomenon. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2010) Consciousness. The science of subjectivity. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Revonsuo, A., & Salmivalli, C
(1995) A content analysis of Bizarre elements in dreams. Dreaming, 5(3), 169–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ricciardi, E., Bonino, D., Sani, L., Vecchi, T., Guazzelli, M., Haxby, J. V., Fadiga, L., & Pietrini, P
(2009) Do we really need vision? How blind people ‘see’ the actions of others. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(31), 9719–9724. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robertson, L. C., & Sagiv, N
(2005) Synesthesia. Perspective from cognitive neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rockwell, T
(2005) Neither ghost nor brain. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
Romney, K., D’Andrade, R. G., & Indow, T
(2005) The distribution of response spectra in the lateral geniculate nucleus compared. PNAS, 102(27), 9720–9725. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rorty, R
(1970) Incorrigibility a S the mark of the mental. Journal of Philosophy, 68, 399–424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rose, N., & Abi-Rached, J. M
(2013) Neuro: The new brain sciences and the management of the mind. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosenberg, A
(2016) Why you don’t know your own mind. The New York Times, July 18.Google Scholar
Russell, B
(1912) The problems of philosophy. London: T. Butterworth.Google Scholar
(1927) An outline of philosophy. London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
(1927) The analysis of matter. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Saadah, E. S., & Melzack, R
(1994) Phantom limb experiences in congenital limb-deficient adults. Cortex, 30(3), 479–485. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sacks, O
(2011) The mind’s eye. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Salminen-Vaparanta, N., Vanni, S., Noreika, V., Valiulis, V., Móró, L., & Revonsuo, A
(2013) Subjective characteristics of TMS-induced phosphenes originating in human V1 and V2. Cerebral Cortex, May 1–10.Google Scholar
Salmon, W. C
(1997) Causality and explanation. Philosophy of Science, 64(3), 461–477. New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Satel, S., & Lilienfeld, S. O
(2013) Brainwashed: The seductive appeal of mindless neuro-science. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Scatena, P
(1990) Phantom representations of congenitally absent limbs. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 70, 1227–1232. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scheiner, C
(1619) Oculus hoc est: Fundamentum opticum. Oeniponti: Apud Danielem.Google Scholar
Schiffer, S
(1990) Physicalism. Philosophical Perspectives, 4(1990), 153–185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schiffman, H. R
(1996) Sensation and perception: An integrated approach. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S
(2003) Are life episodes replayed during dreaming? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(8), 325–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwitzgebel, E
(2002) Why did we think we dreamed in black and white? Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 33(4), 649–660. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Perplexities of consciousness. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
Schwitzgebel, E., Huang, C., & Zhou, Y
(2006) Do we dream in color? Cultural variations and skepticism. Dreaming, 16(1), 36–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seager, W
(2001) Panpsychism. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. E. Zapta (Ed.). Available from [URL]Google Scholar
Searle, J. R
(1983) Intentionality, an essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge (Mass): Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1984) Minds, brains, and science. Cambridge (Mass): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(1992) The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sellars, W
(1962) Philosophy and the scientific image of man. In R. Colodny (Ed.), Frontiers of science and philosophy (pp. 35–78). Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
Seth, A. K., He, B. J., & Hohwy, J
(2015) Neuroscience of consciousness – Editorial. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 1(1), 1–3.Google Scholar
Seth, A. K., Izhikevich, E. M., Ree, G. N., & Edelman, G. M
(2006) Theories and measures of consciousness: An extended framework. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(28), 10799–10804. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shanahan, M. P
(2010) Embodiment and the inner life. Cognition and consciousness in the space of possible minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shannon, C. E
(1948) A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27(July), 379–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shoemaker, S. S
(1990) Qualities and Qualia: What’s in the mind? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 50(May), 109–131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994) Phenomenal character. Noûs, 28(1), 21–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siegel, J. M
(2008) Do all animals sleep? Trends In Neurosciences, 31(4), 208–213. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Sleep viewed as a state of adaptive inactivity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 747–753. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siegel, S
(2006) Direct realism and perceptual consciousness. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, LXXIII(2), 378–410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) The contents of visual experience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sieger, W
(1995) Consciousness, information and panpsychism. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2(3), 272–288.Google Scholar
Silberstein, M
(2010) Essay review. Why neutral monism is superior to panpsychism.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F
(1971) Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Pelican Books.Google Scholar
Skrbina, D
(2003) Panpsychism as an underlying theme in Western philosophy. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10(3), 4–46.Google Scholar
(2005) Panpsychism in the West. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2009) Mind that abides. Panpsychism in the New Millennium. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smart, J. J. C
(1959) Sensations and brain processes. Edited by V. C. Chappell. The Philosophical Review, 68(2), 141–156. Englewood-Cliff (NJ): Prentice Hall. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1978) The content of physicalism. The Philosophical Quarterly, 28(113), 339–341. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, D. A
(2002) The problem of perception. Cambridge (Mass): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(2006) In defence of direct realism. Philosophy and Phenom. Research, 73(2), 411–424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smolin, L
(2013) Temporal naturalism. arXiv: 1310.8539, 1–42.Google Scholar
Solomon, S. G., & Lennie, P
(2007) The machinery of colour vision. Nature, 8, 275–286.Google Scholar
Soon, C. S., Brass, M., Heinze, H.-J., & Haynes, J.-D
(2008) Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, 11(5), 543–545. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
States, B. O
(2000) Dream bizarreness and inner thought. Dreaming, 10(4), 179–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Steiner, P
(2014) Enacting anti-representationalism. The scope and the limits of enactive critiques of representationalism. Avant, 5(2), 43–86.Google Scholar
Stewart, J., Gapenne, O., & Di Paolo, E. A
(2010) Enaction. Edited by J. Stewart, O. Gapenne, & E. A. Di Paolo. Cambridge (Mass): The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stickgold, R. J
(2005) Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature, 437(7063), 1272–1278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stickgold, R. J., & Walker, M. P
(2005) Memory consolidation and reconsolidation: What is the role of sleep? Trends In Neurosciences, 28(8), 408–415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stoughton, C. M., & Conway, B. R
(2008) Neural basis for unique hues. Current Biology, 18(16), 698–699. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Strawson, G
(2005) Why physicalism entails panpsychism. Conference Proceedings. Danish National Research Foundation.Google Scholar
(2006) Does physicalism entail panpsychism? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 13(10–11), 3–31.Google Scholar
(2008) Real materialism and other essays. Oxford: Oxford Clarendon Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stubenberg, L
(1998) Consciousness and Qualia. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suchting, W. A
(1969) Perception and the time-gap argument. The Philosophical Quarterly, 19(74), 46–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Svaetichin, G., & MacNichol, E. F
(1958) Retinal mechanisms for chromatic and achromatic vision. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 74(2), 385–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tauber, A. I
(2010) Freud the reluctant philosopher. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tegmark, M
(2014) Consciousness is a state of matter, like a solid or gas. New Scientist, 222(2964), 28–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Teunisse, R. J., Cruysberg, J. R., Hoefnagels, W. H., Verbeek, A. L., & Zitman, F. G
(1996) Visual hallucinations in psychologically normal Charles Bonnet ’ S syndrome people. The Lancet, 347, 794–797. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, E
(2007) Mind in life. Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Cambridge (Mass): The Belknap Press of the Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, E., & Cosmelli, D
(2011) Brainbound versus enactive views of experience. Philosophical Topics, 39(1), 163–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, E., & Varela, F. J
(2001) Radical embodiment: Neural dynamics and consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(10), 418–425. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tolkien, J. R. R
(1954) The hobbit. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Tononi, G
(2004) An information integration theory of consciousness. BMC Neuroscience, 5(42), 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Consciousness as integrated information: A provisional manifesto. The Biological Bulletin, 215(3), 216–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tononi, G., & Koch, C
(2008) The neural correlates of consciousness: An update. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(March), 239–261. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Consciousness: Here, there but not everywhere. Towards a Science of Consciousness 2014, 1–20.Google Scholar
Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C
(2005) Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1096–1101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tye, M
(1994) Qualia, content, and inverted spectrum. Noûs, 28(2), 159–183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) Representationalism an the transparency of experience. Noûs, 36(1), 137–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Consciousness revisited. Materialism without phenomenal concepts. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
Uttal, W. R
(2001) The new phrenology: The limits of localizing cognitive processes in the brain. Boston: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2004) Dualism. The original sin of cognitivism. Mahwah (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Valberg, A
(2001) Unique hues: An old problem for a new generation. Vision Research, 41(13), 1645–1657. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valberg, J. J
(1992) The puzzle of experience. Oxford: Clarendon Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Boxtel, J. J., Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C
(2010) Opposing effects of attention and consciousness on afterimages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(19), 8883–8888. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Cleve, J
(1990) Mind-dust or magic? Panpsychism versus emergence. Philosophical Perspectives, 4(1990), 215–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Inwagen, P
(1990) Material beings, Vol. 53. New York: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosh, E
(1991) The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.Google Scholar
Velmans, M
(1996) The science of consciousness. Psychological, neuropsychological and clinical reviews. London: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
von Helmholtz, H
(1924a) Treatise on physiological optics. New York: Optical Society of America.Google Scholar
(1924b) Treatise on physiological optics. Menasha (Wisconsin): Optical Society of America.Google Scholar
von Uexküll, J
(1909) Umwelt Und Innenwelt Der Tiere. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
(1957) A stroll through the worlds of animals and men. In C. S. Schiller (Ed.), Instinctive Behavior. The Development of a Modern Concept (pp. 5–80). New York, NY: International University Press.Google Scholar
Wade, N. J
(2009) Beyond body experiences: Phantom limbs, pain and the locus of sensation. Cortex, 45(2), 243–255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleger, R., & Born, J
(2004) Sleep inspires insight. Nature, 427(6972), 352–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Walraven, P. L
(1962) On the mechanisms of colour vision. Thesis, Institute for Perception RVO-TN. Soesterberg, Netherlands.
Weigelt, S., Singer, W., & Muckli, L
(2007) Separate cortical stages in amodal completion revealed by functional magnetic resonance adaptation. BMC Neuroscience, 8(January), 70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Werner, J. S., & Bieber, M. L
(1997) Hue opponency: A constraint on colour categorization. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20(2), 210–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wheatstone, C
(1838) On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 128(1838), 371–394. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wheeler, J. A., & Zurek, W. H
(1983) Quantum theory and measurement. Edited by J. A. Wheeler & W. H. Zurek. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wheeler, R. H
(1918) Visual phenomena in the dreams of a blind subject. Psychological Review, 18(3), 315–312.Google Scholar
Whitaker, H., Smith, C., & Finger, S
(2007) Brain, mind and medicine. Neuroscience in the 18th century. New York: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whitehead, A. N
(1920) Concept of nature. Cambridge (Mass): Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(1925) Science and the modern world. New York: Macmillan Company.Google Scholar
(1929) Process and reality. London: Free Press.Google Scholar
Williams, D. R., & Macleod, D. I. A
(1979) Interchangeable backgrounds for cone afterimages. Vision Research, 19(8), 867–877. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M. H., & Brocklebank, R. W
(1955) Complementary hues of after-images. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 45(4), 293–299. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, R. A
(2004) Boundaries of the mind. The individual in the Fragile Sciences. Cambridge (Mass): Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Yablo, S
(1993) Is conceivability a guide to possibility? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 53(1), 1–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zaidi, Q., Ennis, R., Cao, D., & Lee, B. B
(2012) Neural locus of color afterimages. Current Biology, 22(3), 220–224. Elsevier Ltd. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zeki, S
(2001) Localization and globalization in conscious vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 57–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003) The disunity of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(5), 214–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Splendours and miseries of the brain. Love, creativity and the quest for human happiness. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Zeki, S., & Bartels, A
(1999) Toward a theory of visual consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 8, 225–259. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zubek, J. P
(1969) Sensory deprivation: Fifteen years of research. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar