References
Abrams, R. L., & Christ, S. E.
(2003) Motion onset captures attention. Psychological Science, 14, 427–432.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G.
(2004) Language mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: the blank screen paradigm. Cognition, 93, 79–87.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Altmann, & Kamide
(2003) The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 133–156.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Amati, F. & Brennan, S. E.
(2015) Intention recognition trumps reflexive gaze following. Poster presented at the 27th Annual Convention of the Association for Psychological Science, New York, NY.Google Scholar
Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A.
(2009) Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116(4), 953.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Argyle, M., & Cook, M.
(1976) Gaze and mutual gaze. Oxford, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Astington, J. W., & Gopnik, A.
(1991) Theoretical explanations of children’s understanding of the mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 7–31.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Atalay, A. S., Bodur, H. O., & Rasolofoarison, D.
(2012) Shining in the center: Central gaze cascade effect on product choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(4), 848–866.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon R, Spelke E, Wasserman S.
(1985) Object permanence in 5-month-old infants. Cognition, 20, 191–208.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baker, C. L., Saxe, R., & Tenenbaum, J. B.
(2009) Action understanding as inverse planning. Cognition, 113(3), 329–349.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D. A.
(1995) Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In Moore, C., & Dunham, P., (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
Ballard, D. H., & Hayhoe, M. M.
(2009) Modelling the role of task in the control of gaze. Visual Cognition, 17(6–7), 1185–1204.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S.
(1995) The eye direction detector (EDD) and the shared attention mechanism (SAM): Two cases for evolutionary psychology. In Moore, C., & Dunham, P., (Eds.), Joint attention: its role in development. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
Batki, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Connellan, J., & Ahluwalia, J.
(2000) Is there an innate gaze module? Evidence from human neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 23(2), 223–229.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bayliss, A. P., Schuch, S., & Tipper, S. P.
(2010) Gaze cueing elicited by emotional faces is influenced by affective context. Visual Cognition, 18(8), 1214–1232.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bednarik, R., Vrzakova, H., & Hradis, M.
(2012, March). What do you want to do next: a novel approach for intent prediction in gaze-based interaction. In Proceedings of the Symposium on Eye Tracking Research and Applications (pp.83–90). ACM.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bennewitz, M., Faber, F., Joho, D., & Behnke, S.
(2007) Fritz-A humanoid communication robot. In The 16th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, RO-MAN 2007 (pp.1072–1077).DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bee, N., Prendinger, H., André, E., & Ishizuka, M.
(2006) Automatic preference detection by analyzing the gaze ‘cascade effect’. In Electronic Proceedings of the 2nd conference on communication by gaze interaction, COGAIN (pp.61–64).Google Scholar
Böckler, A., & Sebanz, N.
(2013) Linking joint attention and joint action. In J. Metcalfe & H. Terrace (Eds.), Joint Attention and Metacognition. New York: Oxford University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Böckler, A., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N.
(2011) Observing shared attention modulates gaze following. Cognition, 120(2), 292–298.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borji, A., & Itti, L.
(2014) Defending yarbus: eye movements reveal observers’ task. Journal of Vision, 14(3), 29.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boyle, E., Anderson, A., & Newlands, A.
(1994) The effects of visibility on dialogue and performance in a cooperative problem solving task. Language and Speech, 37, 1–20.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S. E.
(1990) Seeking and providing evidence for mutual understanding. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
(2005) How conversation is shaped by visual and spoken evidence. Approaches to studying world-situated language use: Bridging the language-as-product and language-as-action traditions, 95–129.Google Scholar
Brennan, S. E., Chen, X., Dickinson, C., Neider, M., & Zelinsky, G.
(2008) Coordinating cognition: The costs and benefits of shared gaze during collaborative search. Cognition, 106, 1465–1477.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S. E., & Hulteen, E.
(1995) Interaction and feedback in a spoken language system: A theoretical framework. Knowledge-Based Systems, 8, 143–151.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S. E., & Williams, M.
(1995) The feeling of another’s knowing: Prosody and filled pauses as cues to listeners about the metacognitive states of speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(3), 383–398 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bulling, A., & Gellersen, H.
(2010) Toward mobile eye-based human-computer interaction. Pervasive Computing, IEEE, 9(4), 8–12.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carletta, J., Hill, R. L., Nicol, C., Taylor, T., De Ruiter, J. P., & Bard, E. G.
(2010) Eyetracking for two-person tasks with manipulation of a virtual world. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 254–265.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Castelhano, M. S., Mack, M. L., & Henderson, J. M.
(2009) Viewing task influences eye movement control during active scene perception. Journal of Vision, 9(3):6.1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chandon, P., Hutchinson, J. W., & Young, S. H.
(2001) Measuring the value of point-of-purchase marketing with commercial eye-tracking data. INSEAD.Google Scholar
Churilov, E.
(2014) Speaking, pointing, and gazing: Different communication strategies for different communicative constraints. Unpublished Master’s thesis, Stony Brook University.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Brennan, S. E.
(1991) Grounding in communication. In L. B. Resnick, J. Levine, & S. D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp.127–149). Washington, DC: APA. Reprinted in R. M. Baecker (Ed.), Groupware and computer-supported cooperative work: Assisting human-human collaboration (pp.222–233). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufman Publishers, Inc.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Krych, M. A.
(2004) Speaking while monitoring addressees for understanding. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(1), 62–81.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cline, M. G.
(1967) The perception of where a person is looking. The American Journal of Psychology, 41–50.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crosby, J. R. Monin, B., & Richardson, D.
(2008) Where do we look during potentially offensive behavior? Psychological Science, 19, 226–228).DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeAngelus, M., & Pelz, J. B.
(2009) Top-down control of eye movements: Yarbus revisited. Visual Cognition, 17(6–7), 790–811.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C.
(1987) The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.Google Scholar
Dickie, C., Vertegaal, R., Sohn, C., & Cheng, D.
(2005) eyeLook: using attention to facilitate mobile media consumption. In Proceedings of the 18th annual ACM symposium on User interface software and technology (pp.103–106).Google Scholar
Doherty, M. J.
(2006) The development of mentalistic gaze understanding. Infant and Child Development, 15(2), 179–186.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Doshi, A., & Trivedi, M. M.
(2009) On the roles of eye gaze and head dynamics in predicting driver’s intent to change lanes. Intelligent Transportation Systems, IEEE Transactions on, 10(3), 453–462.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drewes, H., De Luca, A., & Schmidt, A.
(2007) Eye-gaze interaction for mobile phones. In Proceedings of the 4th international conference on mobile technology, applications, and systems and the 1st international symposium on computer human interaction in mobile technology (pp.364–371). ACM.Google Scholar
Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S.
(1999) Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6, 509–540.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fantz, R. L.
(1965) Visual Perception from birth as shown by pattern selectivity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, New Issues in Infant Development, 118, 793–814.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, F., Apel, J., & Henderson, J. M.
(2008) Taking a new look at looking at nothing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 405–410.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foulsham, T., & Lock, M.
(2015) How the eyes tell lies: Social gaze during a preference task. Cognitive Science, 39, 1704–1726.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friesen, C. K. & Kingstone, A.
(1998) The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 490–495.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P.
(2007) Gaze cueing of attention: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 694–724.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frith, C. D., & Frith, U.
(2006) How we predict what other people are going to do. Brain Research, 1079(1), 36–46.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fukusima, S. S., Loomis, J. M., & Da Silva, J. A.
(1997) Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(1), 86.Google Scholar
Glaholt, M. G., & Reingold, E. M.
(2009) Stimulus exposure and gaze bias: A further test of the gaze cascade model. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(3), 445–450.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Eye movement monitoring as a process tracing methodology in decision making research. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 4(2), 125.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Direct control of fixation times in scene viewing: Evidence from analysis of the distribution of first fixation duration. Visual Cognition, 20(6), 605–626.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glaholt, M. G., Wu, M. C., & Reingold, E. M.
(2010) Evidence for top-down control of eye movements during visual decision making. Journal of Vision, 10(5), 15.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, J. H., & Kotval, X. P.
(1999) Computer interface evaluation using eye movements: methods and constructs. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 24(6), 631–645.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, C.
(1981) Conversational organization: Interaction between speakers and hearers. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Bryant, P.
(1997) Words, thoughts, and theories (Vol. 1). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Greene, M. R., Liu, T., & Wolfe, J. M.
(2012) Reconsidering Yarbus: A failure to predict observers’ task from eye movement patterns. Vision Research, 62, 1–8.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P.
(1957) Meaning. Philosophical Review, 66, 377–388.Google Scholar
Haji-Abolhassani, A., & Clark, J. J.
(2014) An inverse Yarbus process: Predicting observers’ task from eye movement patterns. Vision Research, 103, 127–142.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hanna, J. E., & Brennan, S. E.
(2007) Speakers’ eye gaze disambiguates referring expressions early during face-to-face conversation. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(4), 596–615.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henderson, J. M.
(2003) Human gaze control during real-world scene perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 498–504.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henderson, J. M., Shinkareva, S. V., Wang, J., Luke, S. G., & Olejarczyk, J.
(2013) Predicting cognitive state from eye movements. PloS One, 8(5), e64937.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ho, S., Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A.
(2015) Speaking and Listening with the Eyes: Gaze Signaling during Dyadic Interactions. PLoS One, 10(8), e0136905.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J.
(1998) Adults’ eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9(2), 131–134.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Houston-Price, C. & Nakai, S.
(2004) Distinguishing novelty and familiarity effects in infant preference procedures. Infant and Child Development, 13, 341–348.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hunter, M. A. & Ames, E. W.
(1988) A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. In Rovee-Collier C., Lipsitt L. P. (Eds.) Advances in Infancy Research, Vol. 5 (pp.69–95). Stamford, CT: Ablex.Google Scholar
Ishimaru, S., Kunze, K., Kise, K., Weppner, J., Dengel, A., Lukowicz, P., & Bulling, A.
(2014, March). In the blink of an eye: combining head motion and eye blink frequency for activity recognition with Google Glass. In Proceedings of the 5th augmented human international conference (p.15). ACM.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacob, R. J.
(1991) The use of eye movements in human-computer interaction techniques: What you look at is what you get. ACM Transactions on Information Systems (TOIS), 9(3), 152–169.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995) Eye tracking in advanced interface design. In W. Barfield & T. A. Furness (Eds.), Virtual environments and advanced interface design (pp.258–308). New York: OxfordUniversity Press.Google Scholar
Jermann, P., Nüssli, M. A., & Li, W.
(2010, September). Using dual eye-tracking to unveil coordination and expertise in collaborative Tetris. In Proceedings of the 24th BCS Interaction Specialist Group Conference (pp.36–44). British Computer Society.Google Scholar
Ji, Q., Zhu, Z. & Lan, P.
(2004) Real-Time Nonintrusive Monitoring and Prediction of Driver Fatigue. IEEE Transactions on Vehicular Technology, 53, 1052–1068.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P., & Aslin, R.
(1995) Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology 29, 1–23.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kanan, C., Ray, N. A., Bseiso, D. N., Hsiao, J. H., & Cottrell, G. W.
(2014, March). Predicting an observer’s task using multi-fixation pattern analysis. In Proceedings of the Symposium on Eye Tracking Research and Applications (pp.287–290). ACM.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krajbich, I., Armel, C., & Rangel, A.
(2010) Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice. Nature Neuroscience, 13(10), 1292–1298.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krajbich, I., Lu, D., Camerer, C., & Rangel, A.
(2012) The attentional drift-diffusion model extends to simple purchasing decisions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 193.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krajbich, I., & Rangel, A.
(2011) Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(33), 13852–13857.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, G., Tatler, B. W., Findlay, J. M., & Cole, G. G.
(2008) Misdirection in magic: Implications for the relationship between eye gaze and attention. Visual Cognition, 16(2–3), 391–405.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langton, S. R.
(2000) The mutual influence of gaze and head orientation in the analysis of social attention direction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 53(3), 825–845.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langton, S. R. H. & Bruce, V.
(2000) You must see the point: Automatic processing of cues to the direction of social attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 747–757.Google Scholar
Liang, Y., Reyes, M. L., & Lee, J. D.
(2007) Real-time detection of driver cognitive distraction using support vector machines. Intelligent Transportation Systems, IEEE Transactions, 8(2), 340–350.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liebling, D. J., & Preibusch, S.
(2014) Privacy considerations for a pervasive eye tracking world. In Proceedings of the 2014 ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing: Adjunct Publication (pp.1169–1177). ACM.Google Scholar
Loomis, J. M., & Knapp, J. M.
(2003) Visual perception of egocentric distance in real and virtual environments. Virtual and Adaptive Environments, (11), 21–46.Google Scholar
Majaranta, P., & Räihä, K. J.
(2002, March). Twenty years of eye typing: systems and design issues. In Proceedings of the 2002 Symposium on Eye Tracking Research & Applications (pp.15–22). ACM.Google Scholar
Matlock, T., & Richardson, D. C.
(2004) Do eye movements go with fictive motion? Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.909–914). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Maughan, L., Gutnikov, S., & Stevens, R.
(2007) Like more, look more. look more, like more: the evidence from eye-tracking. Journal of Brand Management, 14(4), 335–342.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mills, M., Hollingworth, A., Van der Stigchel, S., Hoffman, L., & Dodd, M. D.
(2011) Examining the influence of task set on eye movements and fixations. Journal of Vision, 11(8), 17.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., & Newell, L.
(2007) Attention, joint attention, and social cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(5), 269–274.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Neider, M. B., Chen, X., Dickinson, C. A., Brennan, S. E., & Zelinsky, G. J.
(2010) Coordinating spatial referencing using shared gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(5), 718–724.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nuku, P., & Bekkering, H.
(2008) Joint attention: Inferring what others perceive (and don’t perceive). Consciousness and Cognition, 17(1), 339–349.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R.
(2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–258.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Patalano, A. L., Juhasz, B. J., & Dicke, J.
(2010) The relationship between indecisiveness and eye movement patterns in a decision making informational search task. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 23(4), 353–368.Google Scholar
Penn, D. C., & Povinelli, D. J.
(2007) On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a ‘theory of mind’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 731–744.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfeiffer, U. J., Vogeley, K., & Schilbach, L.
(2013) From gaze cueing to dual eye-tracking: novel approaches to investigate the neural correlates of gaze in social interaction. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 2516–2528.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pieters, R., & Warlop, L.
(1999) Visual attention during brand choice: The impact of time pressure and task motivation. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 16(1), 1–16.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Posner M. I.
(1980) Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. & Cohen, Y. A.
(1984) Components of visual orienting. In: Bouma, H.; Bouwhuis, D. G. (Eds.) Attention and performance XVII: Control of visual processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1984. pp.531–556.Google Scholar
Reutskaja, E., Nagel, R., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A.
(2011) Search dynamics in consumer choice under time pressure: An eye-tracking study. The American Economic Review, 900–926.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ricciardelli, P., Carcagno, S., Vallar, G., & Bricolo, E.
(2013) Is gaze following purely reflexive or goal-directed instead? Revisiting the automaticity of orienting attention by gaze cues. Experimental Brain Research, 224(1), 93–106.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richardson, D. C., & Dale, R.
(2005) Looking to understand: The coupling between speakers’ and listeners’ eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognitive Science, 29(6), 1045–1060.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Romani, M., Cesari, P., Urgesi, C., Facchini, S., & Aglioti, S. M.
(2003) Motor facilitation of the human cortico-spinal system during observation of bio-mechanically impossible movements. Neuroimage, 26(3), 755–763.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Russo, J. E., & Leclerc, F.
(1994) An eye-fixation analysis of choice processes for consumer nondurables. Journal of Consumer Research, 274–290.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G.
(1974) A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking in conversation. Language, 50, 696–735.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G.
(2006) Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 70–76.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sebanz, N., & Shiffrar, M.
(2009) Bluffing bodies: Inferring intentions from actions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 170–175.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Senju, A., & Csibra, G.
(2008) Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology, 18(9), 668–671.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shiffrar, M., & Freyd, J. J.
(1990) Apparent motion of the human body. Psychological Science, 1(4), 257–264.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shimojo, S., Simion, C., Shimojo, E., & Scheier, C.
(2003) Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference. Nature Neuroscience, 6(12), 1317–1322.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simion, C., & Shimojo, S.
(2006) Early interactions between orienting, visual sampling and decision making in facial preference. Vision Research, 46(20), 3331–3335.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, J. D., & Graham, T. C.
(2006, June). Use of eye movements for video game control. In Proceedings of the 2006 ACM SIGCHI International Conference on Advances in Computer Entertainment Technology (p.20). ACM.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spelke, E. S.
(1990) Principles of object perception. Cognitive Science, 14(1), 29–56.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M. J., & Geng, J. J.
(2001) Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent objects. Psychological Research, 65(4), 235–241.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stein, R. & Brennan, S. E.
(2004) Another person’s eye gaze as a cue in solving programming problems. Proceedings, ICMI 2004, Sixth International Conference on Multimodal Interfaces (pp.9–15), Penn State University, State College, PA.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stiefelhagen, R., & Zhu, J.
(2002, April). Head orientation and gaze direction in meetings. In CHI’02 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp.858–859). ACM.Google Scholar
Suri, R., & Monroe, K. B.
(2003) The effects of time constraints on consumers’ judgments of prices and products. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(1), 92–104 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C.
(1995) Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268(5217), 1632–1634.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Teufel, C., Alexis, D. M., Clayton, N. S., & Davis, G.
(2010) Mental attribution drives rapid, reflexive gaze-following. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 695–705.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(1995) Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore and P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development, 103–130. Laurence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
Tomeo, E., Cesari, P., Aglioti, S. M., & Urgesi, C.
(2012) Fooling the kickers but not the goalkeepers: behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of fake action detection in soccer. Cerebral Cortex, 279, 1–14.Google Scholar
Trösterer, S., Gärtner, M., Wuchse, M., Maurer, B., Baumgartner, A., Meschtscherjakov, A., Tscheligi, M.
(2015) Four eyes see more than two: Shared gaze in the car. Proceedings, Interact 2015.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van der Lans, R. J. A.
(2006) Brand search, (doctoral thesis). Tilburg University, Netherlands.Google Scholar
Velichkovsky, B., Sprenger, A., & Unema, P.
(1997, January). Towards gaze-mediated interaction: Collecting solutions of the “Midas touch problem”. In Human-Computer Interaction INTERACT’97 (pp.509–516). Springer US.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D.
(1990) From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35, 245–275.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., Kawato, M.
(2003) A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Brain and Biological Sciences, 358 (1431), 593–602.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wood, N.
(2014) Autocorrect awareness: categorizing changes and measuring authorial perceptions (unpublished honors thesis). Florida State University, 351, Tallahassee, FL.Google Scholar
Yarbus, A. L.
(1967) Eye movements during perception of complex objects. In Eye movements and vision (pp.171–211). Springer US.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zelinsky, G. J., Dickinson, C. A., Chen, X., Neider, M. B., & Brennan, S. E.
(2005) Collaborative search using shared eye gaze. Abstract, Journal of Vision, 5. p.700.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zelinsky, G. J., Peng, Y., & Samaras, D.
(2013) Eye can read your mind: Decoding gaze fixations to reveal categorical search targets. Journal of Vision, 13(14), 10.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Deppermann, Arnulf, Lorenza Mondada & Simona Pekarek Doehler
2021. Early Responses: An Introduction. Discourse Processes 58:4  pp. 293 ff. DOI logo
Härkki, Tellervo
2023. Mobile gaze tracking and an extended linkography for collaborative sketching and designing. International Journal of Technology and Design Education 33:2  pp. 379 ff. DOI logo
Kiose, Maria I., Anna V. Leonteva, Olga V. Agafonova & Andrey A. Petrov
2023. Multimodal Communicative Moves in Expositive Dialogue: Common and Novel Topic Elaboration. RUDN Journal of Language Studies, Semiotics and Semantics 14:4  pp. 1013 ff. DOI logo
Wagemann, Johannes & Ulrich Weger
2021. Perceiving the Other Self: An Experimental First-Person Account of Nonverbal Social Interaction. The American Journal of Psychology 134:4  pp. 441 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.