Part of
Perspectives on Pantomime
Edited by Przemysław Żywiczyński, Johan Blomberg and Monika Boruta-Żywiczyńska
[Advances in Interaction Studies 12] 2024
► pp. 188216
References (72)
References
Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development, 59(2), 450–466. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (2012). How the brain got language: The mirror system hypothesis. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Young children create iconic gestures to inform others. Developmental Psychology, 50(8), 2049–2060. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bellagamba, F., & Tomasello, M. (1999). Re-enacting intended acts: Comparing 12- and 18-month-olds. Infant Behavior & Development, 22(2), 277–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bigham, S., & Bourchier-Sutton, A. (2007). The decontextualization of form and function in the development of pretence. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 335–351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boyatzis, C. J., & Watson, M. W. (1993). Preschool children’s symbolic representation of objects through gestures. Child Development, 64(3), 729–735. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brentari, D., Coppola, M., Mazzoni, L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). When does a system become phonological? Handshape production in gesturers, signers, and homesigners. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 30(1), 1–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brentari, D., Di Renzo, A., Keane, J., & Volterra, V. (2015). Cognitive, cultural, and linguistic sources of a handshape distinction expressing agentivity. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7(1), 95–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, S., Mittermaier, E., Kher, T., & Arnold, P. (2019). How pantomime works: Implications for theories of language origin. Frontiers in Communication, 4, 9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buxbaum, L. J., Kyle, K. M., & Menon, R. (2005). On beyond mirror neurons: internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 226–239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buxbaum, L. J., Shapiro, A. D., & Coslett, H. B. (2014). Critical brain regions for tool-related and imitative actions: a componential analysis. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 137(pt 7), 1971–1985. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Caselli, M. C., & Volterra, V. (2022). Interaction among modalities and within development. In Gesture in language: Development across the lifespan (pp. 113–133). De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Cristilli, C., de Angelis, V., & Graziano, M. (2011). Learning to use gesture in narratives: Developmental trends in formal and semantic gesture competence. In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating Gestures (pp. 187–200). Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Davis, A. C., & Leach, J. G. (2005). Less Is more: Executive function and symbolic representation in preschool children.” Psychological Science, 16(8), 609–616. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., Stefanini, S., & Volterra, V. (2012). Early action and gesture “vocabulary” and its relation with word comprehension and production. Child Development, 83(2), 526–542. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeLoache, J. S. (1991). Symbolic functioning in very young children: Understanding of pictures and models. Child Development, 62(4), 736–752. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(2), 66–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M., Blasi, D. E., Lupyan, G., Christiansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P. (2015). Arbitrariness, iconicity, and systematicity in language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(10), 603–615. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Edmiston, P., Perlman, M., & Lupyan, G. (2018). Repeated imitation makes human vocalizations more word-like. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1874). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elder, J. L., & Pederson, D. R. (1978). Preschool children’s use of objects in symbolic play. Child Development, 49(2), 500. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
England, M., & Nicoladis, E. (2018). Functional fixedness and body-part-as-object production in pantomime. Acta Psychologica, 190, 174–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., McNeill, D., & Singleton, J. (1996). Silence is liberating: Removing the handcuffs on grammatical expression in the manual modality. Psychological Review, 103(1), 34–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1963). Disturbance of gesture and pantomime in aphasia. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 86, 703–720. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodwyn, S. W., & Acredolo, L. P. (1993). Symbolic gesture versus word: is there a modality advantage for onset of symbol use? Child Development, 64(3), 688–701. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodwyn, S. W., Acredolo, L. P., & Brown, C. A. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(2), 81–103. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M. (1998). Gesture as a communication strategy in second language discourse. Lund University.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1958). A course in modern linguistics. MacMillan.Google Scholar
Hopkins, E. J., Smith, E. D., Weisberg, D. S., & Lillard, A. S. (2016). The development of substitute object pretense: The differential importance of form and function. Journal of Cognition and, 17(2), 197–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hwang, S.-O., Tomita, N., Morgan, H., Ergin, R., İlkbaşaran, D., Seegers, S., Lepic, R., & Padden, C. (2017). Of the body and the hands: patterned iconicity for semantic categories. Language and Cognition, 9(4), 573–602. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Volterra, V., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning to talk in a gesture-rich world: Early communication in Italian vs. American children. First Language, 28(2), 164–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackowitz, E. R., & Watson, M. W. (1980). Development of object transformations in early pretend play. Developmental Psychology, 16(6), 543–549. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnston, J. C., Durieux-Smith, A., & Bloom, K. (2005). Teaching gestural signs to infants to advance child development: A review of the evidence. First Language, 25(2), 235–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, E. F. (1968). Gestural representation of implement usage: An organismic-developmental study [PhD]. Clark University.Google Scholar
Kirk, E., Howlett, N., Pine, K. J., & Fletcher, B. C. (2013). To sign or not to sign? The impact of encouraging infants to gesture on infant language and maternal mind-mindedness. Child Development, 84(2), 574–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Harvard.Google Scholar
Labuschagne, L. G., Cox, T.-J., Brown, K. & Scarf, D. (2017). Too cool? Symbolic but not iconic stimuli impair 4-year-old children’s performance on the delay-of-gratification choice paradigm. Behavioural Processes, 135, 36–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levy, E. & McNeill, D. (2015). Narrative development in young children: Gesture, imagery, and cohesion. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marentette, P., Furman, R., Suvanto, M. E., & Nicoladis, E. (2020). Pantomime (not silent gesture) in multimodal communication: Evidence from children’s narratives. Frontiers in Psychology, 11(November), 575952. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marentette, P., Pettenati, P., Bello, A., & Volterra, V. (2016). Gesture and symbolic representation in Italian and English-speaking Canadian 2-year-olds. Child Development, 87(3), 944–961. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(2012). How language began: Gesture and speech in human evolution. CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). Why we gesture: The surprising role of hand movements in communication. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mineiro, A., Báez-Montero, I. C., Moita, M., Galhano-Rodrigues, I., & Castro-Caldas, A. (2021). Disentangling pantomime from early sign in a new sign language: Window into language evolution research. Frontiers in Psychology, 12(December), 1130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R. W., & Clark, H. (2015). Experimenter’s pantomimes influence children’s use of body part as object and imaginary object pantomimes: A replication. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(5), 703–718. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mizuguchi, T., & Sugai, K. (2002). Object related knowledge and the production of gestures with imagined objects by preschool children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94, 71–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moita, M., Abreu, A. M., & Mineiro, A. (2022). Tracking the evolution of iconic signs to understand the emergence of a phonological system. [Manuscript submitted for publication]. Instituto de Ciências da Saúde, Universidade Católica Portuguesa.Google Scholar
Müller, C. (2014). Gestural modes of representation as techniques of depiction. In C. Müller, A. Cienki, E. Fricke, S. H. Ladewig, D. McNeill, & J. Bressem (Eds.), Body-Language-Communication: An International Handbook on Multimodality in Human Interaction (Vol. 2, pp. 1687–1702). de Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Namy, L. L. (2001). What’s in a name when it isn’t a word? 17-month-olds’ mapping of nonverbal symbols to object categories. Infancy, 2(1), 73–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Reilly, A. W. (1995). Using representations: Comprehension and production of actions with imagined objects. Child Development, 66(4), 999–1010. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ortega, G., & Özyürek, A. (2019). Systematic mappings between semantic categories and types of iconic representations in the manual modality: A normed database of silent gesture. Behavior Research Methods, 52(1), 51–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020). Types of iconicity and combinatorial strategies distinguish semantic categories in silent gesture across cultures. Language and Cognition, 12(1), 84–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Overton, W. F., & Jackson, J. P. (1973). The representation of imagined objects in action sequences: A developmental study. Child Development, 44(2), 309–314. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Padden, C. A., Meir, I., Hwang, S.-O., Lepic, R., Seegers, S., & Sampson, T. (2013). Patterned iconicity in sign language lexicons. Gesture, 13(3), 287–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P., Thompson, R. L., & Vigliocco, G. (2010). Iconicity as a general property of language: Evidence from spoken and signed languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Plays, dreams, and imitation in childhood. Norton.Google Scholar
Schouwstra, M., Swart, H., & Thompson, B. (2019). Interpreting silent gesture: Cognitive biases and rational inference in emerging language systems. Cognitive Science, 43(7), 1–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sonesson, G. (2007). From the meaning of embodiment to the embodiment of meaning: A study in phenomenological semiotics. In T. Ziemke, J. Zlatev, & R. M. Frank (Eds.), Body, Language, and Mind, Vol. 1. Embodiment. (pp. 85–128). De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010). From mimicry to mime by way of mimesis: Reflections on a general theory of iconicity. Sign Systems Studies, 38(1/4), 18–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stokoe, W. C. (1960). Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication systems of the American deaf. Dept. of Anthropology and Linguistics, University of Buffalo.Google Scholar
Striano, T., Tomasello, M., & Rochat, P. (2001). Social and object support for early symbolic play. Developmental Science, 4(4), 442–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Striano, T., & Rochat, P. (1999). Do young children use objects as symbols? The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 563–584. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Troseth, G. L., & DeLoache, J. S. (1998). The medium can obscure the message: Young children’s understanding of video. Child Development, 69(4), 950–965. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vallotton, C. D. (2011). Sentences and conversations before speech? Gestures of preverbal children reveal cognitive and social skills that do not wait for words. In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp. 105–120). Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vallotton, C. D., & Ayoub, C. C. (2010). Symbols build communication and thought: The role of gestures and words in the development of engagement skills and social-emotional concepts during toddlerhood. Social Development, 19(3), 601–626. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Nispen, K., van de Sandt-Koenderman, W. M. E., & Krahmer, E. (2017). Production and comprehension of pantomimes used to depict objects. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(July), 1095. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wacewicz, S., & Żywiczyński, P. (2015). Language evolution: Why Hockett’s design features are a non-starter. Biosemiotics, 8(1), 29–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weidinger, N., Lindner, K., Hogrefe, K., Ziegler, W., & Goldenberg, G. (2017). Getting a grasp on children’s representational capacities in pantomime of object use. Journal of Cognition and Development, 18(2), 246–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation: An organismic-developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. Wiley.Google Scholar
Zlatev, J., Żywiczyński, P., & Wacewicz, S. (2020). Pantomime as the original human-specific communicative system. Journal of Language Evolution, 5(2), 156–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Żywiczyński, P., Wacewicz, S., & Lister, C. (2021). Pantomimic fossils in modern human communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 376(1824), 20200204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (2)

Cited by two other publications

Arbib, Michael
2024. Chapter 1. Pantomime within and beyond the evolution of language. In Perspectives on Pantomime [Advances in Interaction Studies, 12],  pp. 16 ff. DOI logo
Mineiro, Ana & Mara Moita

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 4 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.