Calendar published in:
How the Brain Got Language – Towards a New Road Map
Edited by Michael A. Arbib
[Benjamins Current Topics 112] 2020
► pp. 2237
References

References

Aboitiz, F.
(2012) Gestures, vocalizations and memory in language origins. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 4(2). Crossref.Google Scholar
(2013) How did vocal behavior “take over” the gestural communication system? Language and Cognition, 5, 167–176. Crossref[ p. 35 ]Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A.
(2012) How the Brain Got Language: The Mirror System Hypothesis. New York & Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Complex Imitation and the Language-Ready Brain. Language and Cognition, 5(2–3), 273–312. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2016) Towards a Computational Comparative Neuroprimatology: Framing the Language-Ready Brain. Physics of Life Reviews, 16, 1–54. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2017) Dorsal and ventral streams in the evolution of the language-ready brain: Linking language to the world. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 43, Part B, 228–253. Crossref.Google Scholar
(2018) Computational challenges of evolving the language-ready brain: 1. From Manual Action to Protosign. Interaction Studies, 19(1–2), 7–21. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A. & Bonaiuto, J. J.
(eds) (2016) From Neuron to Cognition via Computational Neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. & Caplan, D.
(1979) Neurolinguistics must be Computational. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 449–483. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A., Ganesh, V. & Gasser, B.
(2014a) Dyadic Brain Modeling, Ontogenetic Ritualization of Gesture in Apes, and the Contributions of Primate Mirror Neuron Systems. Phil Trans Roy Soc B, 369 (1644), 20130414. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A., Gasser, B. & Barrès, V.
(2014b) Language is handy but is it embodied? Neuropsychologia, 55, 57–70. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A. & Lee, J. Y.
(2008) Describing visual scenes: Towards a neurolinguistics based on construction grammar. Brain Research, 1225, 146–162. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Template Construction Grammar and the Description of Visual Scenes. The Neurobiology of Language Conference, Chicago.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. & Liaw, J. -S.
(1995) Sensorimotor Transformations in the Worlds of Frogs and Robots. Artificial Intelligence, 72, 53–79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baker, M.
(2001) The Atoms of Language: The Mind’s Hidden Rules of Grammar. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Barrès, V. & Arbib, M. A.
(2018a) From Gaze Patterns to Utterances: Modeling the Dynamics of Visual Scene Description. Cognitive Science, In preparation.Google Scholar
(2018b) SALVIA: A Neuro-Cognitive Model of Normal and Agrammatic Language Comprehension. Brain and Language, In preparation.Google Scholar
Barrès, V. & Lee, J. Y.
(2014) Template Construction Grammar: From Visual Scene Description to Language Comprehension and Agrammatism. Neuroinformatics, 12(1), 181–208. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Behrmann, M. & Plaut, D. C.
(2013) Distributed circuits, not circumscribed centers, mediate visual recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(5), 210–219. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Berwick, R. C. & Chomsky, N.
(2016) Why only us: Language and Evolution, Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D.
(1995) Language and Human Behavior. Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
(2009) Adam’s Tongue. How Humans Made Language, How Language Made Humans. New York: Hill & Wang.[ p. 36 ]Google Scholar
Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. & Schlesewsky, M.
(2013) Reconciling time, space and function: A new dorsal–ventral stream model of sentence comprehension. Brain Lang, 125(1), 60–76. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cangelosi, A. & Parisi, D.
(eds) (2002) Simulating the Evolution of Language. London: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, A. & Zurif, E. B.
(1976) Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 3(4), 572–582. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F.
(2015) The role of learning in theories of English and Japanese sentence processing, in Nakyama, M. (ed), Handbook of Japanese psycholinguistics. Boston: De Gruyter Mouton, 353–385. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Colagé, I.
(2016) The Cultural Evolution of Language and Brain: Comment on “Towards a computational comparative neuroprimatology: Framing the language-ready brain” by M.A. Arbib. Physics of Life Reviews, 16, 61–62. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L.
(2011) The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 15(6), 254–262. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Draper, B. A., Collins, R. T., Brolio, J., Hanson, A. R. & Riseman, E. M.
(1989) The schema system. International Journal of Computer Vision, 2, 209–250. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fagg, A. H. & Arbib, M. A.
(1998) Modeling parietal-premotor interactions in primate control of grasping. Neural Netw, 11(7–8), 1277–1303. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fogassi, L., Coudé, G. & Ferrari, P. F.
(2013) The extended features of mirror neurons and the voluntary control of vocalization in the pathway to language. Language and Cognition, 5, 145–155. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D.
(2011) The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiological Reviews, 91(4), 1357–1392. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hagoort, P.
(2013) MUC (Memory, Unification, Control) and beyond. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. Crossref.Google Scholar
Hecht, E. E., Gutman, D. A., Bradley, B. A., Preuss, T. M. & Stout, D.
(2015) Virtual dissection and comparative connectivity of the superior longitudinal fasciculus in chimpanzees and humans. Neuroimage, 108, 124–137. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hickok, G. & Poeppel, D.
(2007) The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat Rev Neurosci, 8(5), 393–402. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hill, J. C.
(1983) A computational model of language acquisition in the two-year-old. Cognition and Brain Theory, 6, 287–317.Google Scholar
Kirby, S.
(2000) Syntax without natural selection: How compositionality emerges from vocabulary in a population of learners, in Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Hurford, J. R. (eds), The evolutionary emergence of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 99–119. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kirby, S., Cornish, H. & Smith, K.
(2008) Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(31), 10681–10686. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W.
(2006) How New Languages Emerge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref[ p. 37 ]Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(1987) The Competition Model, in MacWhinney, B. (ed), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 249–308.Google Scholar
(2005) A unified model of language development, in Kroll, J. F. & Groot, A. M. B. D. (eds), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 49–67.Google Scholar
(2014) Item-based patterns in early syntactic development, in Herbst, T., Schmid, H. -J. & Faulhaber, S. (eds), Constructions Collocations Patterns Walter de Gruyter, 33–69.Google Scholar
Menenti, L., Gierhan, S. M. E., Segaert, K. & Hagoort, P.
(2011) Shared Language Overlap and Segregation of the Neuronal Infrastructure for Speaking and Listening Revealed by Functional MRI. Psychological Science, 22(9), 1173–1182. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Myowa, M.
(2018) The Evolutionary Roots of Human Imitation, Action Representation, and Word Learning Interaction Studies, 19(1–2), 183–199. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rilling, J. K., Glasser, M. F., Preuss, T. M., Ma, X., Zhao, T., Hu, X. & Behrens, T. E.
(2008) The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nature Neuroscience, 11(4), 426–428. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sandler, W., Aronoff, M., Meir, I. & Padden, C.
(2011) The gradual emergence of phonological form in a new language. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory. Crossref.Google Scholar
Segaert, K., Menenti, L., Weber, K., Petersson, K. M. & Hagoort, P.
(2012) Shared Syntax in Language Production and Language Comprehension – An fMRI Study. Cerebral Cortex, 22(7), 1662–1670. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Steels, L.
(2011) Modeling the cultural evolution of language. Physics of Life Reviews, 8(4), 339–356. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wray, A.
(1998) Protolanguage as a holistic system for social interaction. Language & Communication, 18, 47–67. CrossrefGoogle Scholar