References
Anderson, D., Magill, R., & Sekiya, H.
(2001) Motor learning as a function of KR schedule and characteristics of task-intrinsic feedback. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33(1), 59–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, D., Magill, R., Sekiya, H., & Ryan, G.
(2005) Support for an explanation of the guidance effect in motor skill learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37(3), 231–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arroyo, D. C., & Yilmaz, Y.
(2018) An open for replication study: The role of feedback timing in synchronous computer-mediated communication. Language Learning, 68(4), 942–972. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.
(2000) The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G.
(1974) Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Case, R., Kurland, M., & Goldberg, J.
(1982) Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 386–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Conlin, J., Gathercole, S., & Adams, J.
(2005) Children’s working memory: Investigating performance limitations in complex span tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90(4), 303–317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Conway, A., Kane, M., Bunting, M., Hambrick, D., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R.
(2005) Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 769–786. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R.
(2007) Practice in a second language: Perspectives from applied linguistics and cognitive psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Interactions between individual differences, treatments, and structures in SLA. Language Learning, 62(Suppl. 2), 189–200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Doughty, C., & Varela, E.
(1998) Communicative focus on form. In C. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 114–138). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, R.
(2005) Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(02), 141–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) The differential effects of corrective feedback on two grammatical structures. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A collection of empirical studies (pp. 339–360). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Erlam, R., & Loewen, S.
(2010) Implicit and explicit recasts in L2 oral French interaction. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 66(6), 887–916. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S., Pickering, S., Knight, C., & Stegmann, Z.
(2004) Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goo, J.
(2012) Corrective feedback and working memory capacity in interaction-driven L2 learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(3), 445–474. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kim, J., & Han, Z.
(2007) Recasts in communicative EFL classes: Do teacher intent and learner interpretations overlap? In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A collection of empirical studies (pp. 269–297). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kim, Y., Payant, C., & Pearson, P.
(2015) The intersection of task-based interaction, task complexity, and working memory. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37, 549–581. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, C., & Thompson, S.
(1981) A functional reference grammar of Mandarin Chinese. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Li, S.
(2013a) The interactions between the effects of implicit and explicit feedback and individual differences in language analytic ability and working memory. Modern Language Journal, 97, 634–654. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013b) The differential roles of language analytic ability and working memory in mediating the effects of two types of feedback on the acquisition of an opaque linguistic structure. In C. Sanz & B. Lado (Eds.), Individual differences, L2 development & language program administration: From theory to application (pp. 32–52). Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Li, S., Zhu, Y., & Ellis, R.
(2016) The effects of the timing of corrective feedback on the acquisition of a new linguistic structure. The Modern Language Journal, 100(1), 276–295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, S.
(2017) The effects of cognitive aptitudes on the process and product of L2 interaction. In L. Gurzynski-Weiss (Ed.), Expanding individual difference research in the interaction approach: Investigating learners, instructors, and other interlocutors (pp. 41–70). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, S., & Fu, M.
(2018) Strategic and unpressured within-task planning and their associations with working memory. Language Teaching Research, 22(2), 230–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, S., Ellis, R., & Zhu, Y.
(2019) The associations between cognitive ability and L2 development under five different instructional conditions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1–30.Google Scholar
Linck, J., Osthus, P., Koeth, J., & Bunting, M.
(2014) Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 861–883. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mackey, A., Philp, J., Egi, T., Fuji, A., & Tatsumi, T.
(2002) Individual differences in working memory, noticing of feedback, and L2 development. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Individual differences and instructed language learning (pp. 181–209). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mackey, A., & Sachs, R.
(2012) Older learners in SLA research: A first look at working memory, feedback, and L2 development. Language Learning, 62, 704–740. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mifka-Profozic, N.
(2013) The effectiveness of corrective feedback and the role of individual differences in language learning. Frankfurt: Peter Lang. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Quinn, P.
(2014) Delayed Versus Immediate Corrective Feedback on Orally Produced Passive Errors in English (Unpublished PhD dissertation). University of Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
Révész, A.
(2012) Working memory and the observed effectiveness of recasts on different L2 outcome measures. Language Learning, 62, 93–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roche, K., & Chainay, H.
(2013) Visually guided grasping of common objects: Effects of priming. Visual Cognition, 21(8), 1010–1032. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salmoni, A., Schmidt, R., & Walter, C.
(1984) Knowledge of results and motor learning: A review and critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 95(3), 355–386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanz, C., Lin, H., Lado, B., Stafford, C., & Bowden, H.
(2014) One size fits all? Learning conditions and working memory capacity in ab initio language development. Applied Linguistics, 37(5), 669–692. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R., Young, D., Swinnen, S., & Shapiro, D.
(1989) Summary knowledge of results for skill acquisition: Support for the guidance hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(2), 352–359.Google Scholar
Schooler, L., & Anderson, J.
(2008) The disruptive potential of immediate feedback. Retrieved from [URL]
Skinner, B.
(1954) The science of learning and the art of teaching. Harvard Educational Review, 24(2), 86–97.Google Scholar
Spada, N., Shiu, J., & Tomita, Y.
(2015) Validating an elicited imitation task as a measure of implicit knowledge: Comparisons with other validation studies. Language Learning, 65(3), 723–751. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wells, E., Kofler, M., Soto, E., Schaefer, H., & Sarver, D.
(2018) Assessing working memory in children with ADHD: Minor administration and scoring changes may improve digit span backward’s construct validity. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 72, 166–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, Y., & Lyster, R.
(2010) Effects of form-focused practice and feedback on Chinese EFL learners’ acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32(02), 235–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yilmaz, Y.
(2013) Relative effects of explicit and implicit feedback: The role of working memory and language analytic ability. Applied Linguistics, 34, 344–368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhao, Y.
(2015) The effects of explicit and implicit recasts on the acquisition of two grammatical structures and the mediating role of working memory (Unpublished PhD dissertation). The University of Auckland, New Zealand.Google Scholar