Part of
Phonological and Phonetic Considerations of Lexical Processing
Edited by Gonia Jarema and Gary Libben
[Benjamins Current Topics 80] 2015
► pp. 6786
References
Berent, I., & Pinker, S
Bernhardt, B.H., & Stemberger, J.P
(1998) Handbook of phonological development: From the perspective of constraint-based nonlinear phonology. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bleile, K.M
(1987) Regressions in the phonological development of two children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa.Google Scholar
Boersma, P., & Hayes, B
(2001) Empirical tests of the gradual learning algorithm. Linguistic Inquiry, 32, 45–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Browman, C.P., & Goldstein, L.M
(1986) Towards an articulatory phonology. Phonology Yearbook, 3, 219–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buck-Gengler, C.J., Menn, L., & Healy, A
(2004) What ‘‘mice-trap’’ tells us about the mental lexicon. Brain and Language, 90, 453–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fromkin, V.A
(1971) The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language, 47, 27–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerken, L
(1991) The metrical basis for children’s subjectless sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 431–451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, M., & Crago, M.B
(1991) Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition, 39, 1–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, M., & Goad, H
(1997) What underlies inflectional error patterns in Genetic Dysphasia? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 10, 109–137. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, P
(1985) Level ordering in lexical development. Cognition, 21, 73–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haskell, T.R., MacDonald, M.C., & Seidenberg, M.S
(2003) Language learning and innateness: Some implications of compounds research. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 119–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hayes, B
(1999) Phonetically-driven phonology: The role of optimality theory and inductive grounding. InM. Darnell, E. Moravscik, M. Noonan, F. Newmeyer, & K. Wheatly(Eds.), Functionalism and formalism in linguistics, vol. 1 (pp. 243–285). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M., & Stoel-Gammon, C
(1997) Truncation patterns in English-speaking children’s word productions. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 40, 526–541. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, P
(1982) From cyclic phonology to lexical phonology. InH. van der Hulst & N. Smith(Eds.), The structure of phonological representations, part 1 (pp. 130–175). Dordrecht, Holland: Foris.Google Scholar
Klein, H.B., & Liu-Shea, M
(2009) Between-word simplification patterns in the continuous speech of children with speech sound disorders. Language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 40, 17–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B
(2000) The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. Third Edition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Marshall, C.R., & Van der Lely, H.K.J
(2006) A challenge to current models of past tense inflection: the impact of phonotactics. Cognition, 100, 302–320. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Menn, L., & Matthei, E
(1992) The “two-lexicon” approach of child phonology: Looking back, looking ahead. InC.A. Ferguson, L. Menn, & C. Stoel-Gammon(Eds.), Phonological development: Models, research, implications (pp. 211–248). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S., & Prince, A.S
(1988) On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S., & Ullman, M.T
(2002) The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 456–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prince, A., & Smolensky, P
(2004) Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramscar, M., & Dye, M
(2010) Learning language from the input: Why innate constraints can’t explain noun compounding. Cognitive Psychology, 62, 1–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smolensky, P
(1999) Grammar-based connectionist approaches to language. Cognitive Science, 23, 589–613. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stemberger, J.P
(1993) Glottal transparency. Phonology, 10, 107–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Children’s overtensing errors: Phonological and lexical effects on syntax. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 49–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Storkel, H.L., Armbrüster, J., & Hogan, T.P
(2006) Differentiating phonotactic probability and neighborhood density in adult word learning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 1175–1192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Storkel, H.L., & Rogers, M.A
(2000) The effect of probabilistic phonotactics on lexical acquisition. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 14, 407–425. DOI logoGoogle Scholar