Part of
Phonological and Phonetic Considerations of Lexical Processing
Edited by Gonia Jarema and Gary Libben
[Benjamins Current Topics 80] 2015
► pp. 87100
References (35)
Brand, M., Rey, A., & Peereman, R. (2003). Where is the syllable priming effect in visual word recognition? Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 435–443. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cho, M.-H. (2004). Rhythm typology of Korean speech. Cognitive Processing, 5, 249–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., Woollams, A., Kinoshita, S., & Perry, C. (1999). A position-sensitive Stroop effect: Further evidence for a left-to-right component in print-to-speech conversion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 456–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia, J.A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Influence of prime lexicality, frequency, and pronounceability on the masked onset priming effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1813–1837. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferrand, L., Segui, J., & Grainger, J. (1996). Masked priming of word and picture naming: The role of syllabic units. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 708–723. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K.I., & Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 680–698. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1991). The density constraint on form-priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K.I., & Forster, J.C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 35, 116–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K.I., Mohan, K., & Hector, J. (2003). The mechanics of masked priming. InS. Kinoshita & S.J. Lupker(Eds.), Masked priming: The state of the art (pp. 3–37). Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Grainger, J., & Ferrand, L. (1996). Masked orthographic and phonological priming in visual word recognition and naming: Cross-task comparisons. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 623–647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Han, M. (1964). Duration of Korean vowels. Studies in the Phonology of Asian Languages II. Acoustic Phonetics Research Laboratory, University of Southern California.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. (1989). Compensatory lengthening in moraic phonology. Linguistic Inquiry, 20, 253–306.Google Scholar
Jun, J. (1994). Metrical weight consistency in Korean partial reduplication. Phonology, 11, 69–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kim, J., & Davis, C. (2002). Using Korean to investigate phonological priming effects without the influence of orthography. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 569–591. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kinoshita, S. (2000). The left-to-right nature of the masked onset priming effect in naming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 133–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2003). The nature of masked onset priming effects in naming: A review. InS. Kinoshita & S.J. Lupker(Eds.), Masked priming: The state of the art (pp. 223–238). Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Kinoshita, S., & Woollams, A. (2002). The masked onset priming effect in naming: Computation of phonology or speech planning? Memory and Cognition, 30, 237–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kubozono, H. (1985). Speech errors and syllable structure. Linguistics and Philology, 6, 220– 243.Google Scholar
. (1989). The mora and syllable structure in Japanese: Evidence from speech errors. Language and Speech, 32, 249–278.Google Scholar
Kureta, Y., Fushimi, T., & Tatsumi, I.F. (2006). The functional unit of phonological encoding:Evidence for moraic representation in native Japanese speakers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1102–1119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, C.H., & Taft, M. (2009). Are onsets and codas important in processing letter position? A comparison of TL effects in English and Korean. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 530–542. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Malouf, T., & Kinoshita, S. (2007). Masked onset priming effect for high-frequency words: Further support for the speech-planning account. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1155–1167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mousikou, P., Coltheart, M., Finkbeiner, M., & Saunders, S. (2010). Can the dual-route cascaded computational model of reading offer a valid account of the masked onset priming effect? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 984–1003. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mousikou, P., Coltheart, M., & Saunders, S. (2010). Computational modelling of the masked onset priming effect in reading aloud. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 725–763. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Otake, T., Hatano, G., Cutler, A., & Mehler, J. (1993). Mora or syllable? Speech segmentation in Japanese. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 258–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Port, R.F., Dalby, J., & O’Dell, M. (1987). Evidence for mora-timing in Japanese. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 81, 1574–1585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Protopapas, A. (2007). CheckVocal: A program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response times of vocal responses from DMDX. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 859–862. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schiller, N.O. (1998). The effect of visually masked syllable primes on the naming latencies of words and pictures. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 484–507. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1999). Masked syllable priming in English nouns. Brain and Language, 68, 300–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2000). Single word production in English: The role of subsyllabic units during phonological encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 512–528. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2004). The onset effect in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 477–490. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2008). The masked onset priming effect in picture naming. Cognition, 106, 952–962. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verdonschot, R.G., Kiyama, S., Tamaoka, K., Kinoshita, S., La Heij, W., & Schiller, N.O. (2011). The functional unit of Japanese word naming: Evidence from masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1458–1473. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Warner, N., & Arai, T. (2001). Japanese mora-timing: A review. Phonetica, 58, 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar