Article published In:
Computational Construction Grammar and Constructional Change
Edited by Katrien Beuls and Remi van Trijp
[Belgian Journal of Linguistics 30] 2016
► pp. 193217
References
Bentley, R. Alexander, Paul Ormerod, and Stephen Shennan
2011 “Population-Level Neutral Model Already Explains Linguistic Patterns.” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278 (1713): 1770–1772. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Benz, Anton, Gerhard Jäger, and Robert van Rooij
(eds) 2006Game Theory and Pragmatics. New York: Palgrave MacMillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chudek, Maciej, Sarah Heller, Susan Birch, and Joseph Henrich
2012 “Prestige-Biased Cultural Learning: Bystander’s Differential Attention to Potential Models Influences Children’s Learning.” Evolution and Human Behavior 33 (1): 46–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fagyal, Zsuzsanna, Samarth Swarup, Anna Maria Escobar, Les Gasser, and Kiran Lakkaraju
2010 “Centers, Peripheries, and Popularity: The Emergence of Norms in Simulated Networks of Linguistic Influence.” In University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, Volume 15, Chapter 10, 81–90. Philadelphia.Google Scholar
Gansner, Emden R. and Stephen C. North
2000 “An Open Graph Visualization System and its Applications to Software Engineering.” Software – Practice and Experience 30 (11): 1203–1233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, Nigel
2008Agent-Based Models. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, Thomas L. and Florencia Reali
2011 “Modelling minds as well as populations.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 278 (1713): 1773–1776. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Han, Matthew W. and R. Alexander Bentley
2003 “Drift as a Mechanism for Cultural Change: An Example from Baby Names.” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 270 (Suppl. 1): 120–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin
1999 “Why is Grammaticalization Irreversible?Linguistics 37 (6): 1043–1068. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, Matthew O.
2008Social and Economic Networks. Princeton: Princeton University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kusters, Wouter
2003Linguistic Complexity. The Influence of Social Change on Verbal Inflection. Ph. D. thesis, Universiteit Leiden, Leiden.Google Scholar
2008 “Prehistoric and Posthistoric Language in Oblivion.” In Variation, Selection, Development. Probing the Evolutionary Model of Language Change, ed. by Regine Eckardt, Gerhard Jäger, and Tonjes Veenstra, Trends in Linguistics, 199–218. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Mühlenbernd, Roland and Michael Franke
2012 “Signaling Conventions: Who Learns What Where and When in a Social Network.” In The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of EvoLang9, ed. by Thomas C. Scott-Philips, Monica Tamariz, Erica A. Cartmill, and James R. Hurford, 242–249. Singapore: World Scientific. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paul, Hermann
1995Prinzipien der Sprachgeschichte (9 ed.). Tübingen: Niemeyer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B., Forrest Stonedahl, and Robert Daland
2014 “A model of grassroots changes in linguistic systems.” CoRR abs/1408 1985.Google Scholar
Pijpops, Dirk, Katrien Beuls, and Freek Van de Velde
2015 “The Rise of the Verbal Weak Inflection in Germanic. An Agent-Based Model.” Computational Linguistics in the Netherlands Journal 51: 81–102.Google Scholar
Pustejovsky, James
1995The Generative Lexicon. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
R Core Team
2013R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Reali, Florencia and Thomas L. Griffiths
2009a “The Evolution of Frequency Distributions: Relating Regularization to Inductive Biases through Iterated Learning.” Cognition 1111: 317–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2009b “Words as alleles: connecting language evolution with Bayesian learners to models of genetic drift.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 277 (1680): 429–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schaden, Gerhard
2014 “Markedness, Frequency and Lexical Change in Unstable Environments.” In Proceedings of the Formal & Experimental Pragmatics Workshop, ed. by Judith Degen, Michael Franke, and Noah Goodman, 43–50. Tübingen.Google Scholar
Skyrms, Brian
2010Signals. Evolution, Learning, & Information. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, Kenny, Simon Kirby, and Henry Brighton
2003 “Iterated Learning: A Framework for the Emergence of Language.” Artificial Life 91: 371–386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sutcliffe, Alistair, Robin Ian McDonald Dunbar, Jens Binder, and Holly Arrow
2012 “Relationships and the Social Brain: Integrating Psychological and Evolutionary Perspectives.” British Journal of Psychology 103 (2): 149–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar