References

References

Allen, L. Q.
(2000) Form-Meaning Connections and the French Causative. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 22, 69–84. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Angelovska, T., & Roehm, D.
(2020) Processing instruction and the effects of input modality on morphosyntactic acquisition of English as L2: An Austrian school-lab classroom study. Instructed Second Language Acquisition, 4, 180–201. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Benati, A.
(2004a) The effects of structured input and explicit information on the acquisition of Italian future tense. In B. VanPatten (Ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary (pp. 207–255). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2004b) The effects of processing instruction and its components on the acquisition of gender agreement in Italian. Language Awareness, 13, 67–80. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) The effects of PI, TI and MOI in the acquisition of English simple past tense. Language Teaching Research, 9, 67–113. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013a) The Input Processing Theory. In P. Garcia Mayo, Contemporary approaches to second language acquisition (pp. 93–110). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013b) Age and the effects of processing instruction on the acquisition of English Passive constructions among school children and adult native speakers of Turkish. In J. F. Lee & A. Benati (Eds.), Individual differences and processing instruction (pp. 83–104). Equinox.Google Scholar
(2019) Classroom-oriented research: Processing Instruction. Language Teaching, 52(3), 343–359. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2020a) The effects of structured input and traditional instruction on the acquisition of the English causative passive forms: An eye-tracking study measuring accuracy in responses and processing patterns. Language Teaching Research, 24, 1–21. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2020b) An eye-tracking study on the effects of structured input and traditional instruction on the acquisition of English passive constructions: An eye tracking study. Instructed Second Language Acquisition, 4, 158–179. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Benati, A., & Lee, J.
(2008) Grammar acquisition and processing instruction: Secondary and cumulative effects. Multilingual Matters. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Processing instruction and discourse. Continuum.Google Scholar
(Eds.) (2015) Processing instruction: New insights after twenty years of theory, research and application. Special issue in International Review of Applied Linguistics (IRAL), 53(2).Google Scholar
Benati, A., & Batziou, M.
(2017) The effects of structured-input and structured-output tasks on the acquisition of English causative. International Review of Applied Linguistics (IRAL), 57, 265–288. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Benati, A., & Schwieter, J.
(2017) Input processing and processing instruction: Pedagogical and cognitive considerations for L3 acquisition In A. Tanja & A. Hahn (Eds.), L3 syntactic transfer: Models, new developments and implications (pp. 195–223). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Benati, A., & Batziou, M.
(2019) Discourse and long-term effects of structured-input and structured-output tasks in combination and isolation on the acquisition of passive English causative forms. Language Awareness, 28(2) 1–18. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chiuchiu, G., & Benati, A.
(2020) A self-paced-reading study on the effects of structured input and textual enhancement on the acquisition of the Italian subjunctive of doubt. Instructed Second Language Acquisition, 4, 235–257. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M., & Sokalski, K. J.
(1996) The differential role of comprehension and production practice. Language Learning 46, 613–642. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Farley, A.
(2004) Processing instruction and the Spanish subjunctive: Is explicit information needed? In B. VanPatten (Ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary (pp. 227–239). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Henry, N.
(2015) Morphosyntactic processing, cue interaction, and the effects of instruction: An investigation of processing instruction and the acquisition of case markings in L2 German (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA.
Ito, K., & Wong, W.
(2019) Processing instruction and the effects of input modality and voice familiarity on the acquisition of the French causative construction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41, 443–468. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Laval, C., & Lowe, H.
(2020) The use of eye-tracking in experimental approaches in second language acquisition research: The primary effects of processing instruction in the acquisition of the French imperfect. Journal of French Language Studies, 30, 189–210. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lee, J.
(2015) The milestones in twenty years of processing instruction research. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 53, 111–126. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lee, J., & Benati, A.
(2007a) Delivering processing instruction in classrooms and virtual contexts: Research and practice. Equinox.Google Scholar
(2007b) Second language processing: An analysis of theory, problems and possible solutions. Continuum.Google Scholar
(2009) Research and perspectives on processing instruction. Mouton de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lee, J., & Doherty, S.
(2018) Native and nonnative processing of active and passive sentence: The effects of processing instruction on the allocation of visual attention. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 42, 853–879.Google Scholar
Lee, J., Malovrh, P., Doherty, S., & Nichols, A.
(2020) A self-paced reading study of the effects of processing instruction on the L2 processing of active and passive sentences. Language Teaching Research, 53, 127–150. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Long, M.
(1983) Does second language instruction make a difference? TESOL Quarterly, 17, 359–82. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Norris, J., & Ortega, L.
(2000) Effectiveness of second language instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta-analysis. Language Learning, 50, 417–528. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Salaberry, M. R.
(1997) The role of input and output practice in second language acquisition. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 53, 422–451. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B.
(1996) Input processing and grammar instruction: Theory and research. Ablex.Google Scholar
(2004) Input processing in second language acquisition. In B. VanPatten (Ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary (pp. 5–31). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2015a) Input processing in adult SLA. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (2nd ed., pp.113–134). Routledge.Google Scholar
(2015b) Foundations of processing instruction. International Review of Applied Linguistics (IRAL), 53, 91–109. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B., & Benati, A.
(2015) Key terms in second language acquisition. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B., & Cadierno, T.
(1993) Explicit instruction and input processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 15, 225–243. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B., & Oikennon, S.
(1996) Explanation vs. structured input in processing instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 495–510. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B., & Rothman, J.
(2014) Against rules. In A. Benati, C. Laval & M. Arche (eds.), The grammar dimension in instructed second language learning (15–35). London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B., & Wong, W.
(2004) Processing instruction and the French causative: Another replication. In B. VanPatten (Ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary (pp. 97–118). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B., & Fernández, C.
(2004) The long-term effects of processing instruction. In B. VanPatten (Ed.), Processing Instruction: Theory, Research, and Commentary (pp. 273–289). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B., Farmer, J., & Clardy, C.
(2009) Processing instruction and meaning- based output instruction: A response to Keating and Farley (2008). Hispania, 92, 116–126.Google Scholar
Wong, W., & Ito, K.
(2018) The effects of processing instruction and traditional instruction on L2 online processing of the causative construction in French: An eye-tracking study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40, 241–268. CrossrefGoogle Scholar