Chapter published in:
Prediction in Second Language Processing and Learning
Edited by Edith Kaan and Theres Grüter
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 12] 2021
► pp. 2646
References

References

Bargh, J. A.
(1994) The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition: Basic processes; Applications (pp. 1–40). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bobb, S. C., Kroll, J. F., & Jackson, C. N.
(2015) Lexical constraints in second language learning: Evidence on grammatical gender in German. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(3), 502–523. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K.
(1982) Toward a cognitive psychology of syntax: Information processing contributions to sentence formulation. Psychological Review, 89(1), 1–47. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brothers, T., Swaab, T. Y., & Traxler, M. J.
(2017) Goals and strategies influence lexical prediction during sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 203–216. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, C. G., & Cooke, H.
(2009) Lexical competition during second-language listening: Sentence context, but not proficiency, constrains interference from the native lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(4), 1029–1040. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chun, E., & Kaan, E.
(2019) L2 prediction during complex sentence processing. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 3(2), 203–216. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cook, A. E., & Meyer, A. S.
(2008) Capacity demands of phoneme selection in word production: New evidence from dual-task experiments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(4), 886–899. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Corps, R. E., Brooke, C., & Pickering, M. J.
(2021) Do comprehenders predict from their own perspective? Manuscript in preparationGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Heij, W., & La Navarrete, E.
(2006) The dynamics of bilingual lexical access. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9(2), 137. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M.
(2004) Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(4), 491–511. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Curcic, M., Andringa, S., & Kuiken, F.
(2019) The role of awareness and cognitive aptitudes in L2 predictive language processing. Language Learning, 69, 42–71. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Declerck, M., & Kormos, J.
(2012) The effect of dual task demands and proficiency on second language speech production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(4), 782–796. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S.
(1986) A spreading-activation theory of retreival in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93(3), 283–321. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M.
(2005) Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1117–1121. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dijkgraaf, A., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Duyck, W.
(2017) Predicting upcoming information in native-language and non-native-language auditory word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(5), 917–930. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2019) Prediction and integration of semantics during L2 and L1 listening. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(7), 881–900. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Valdés Kroff, J. R., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., & Gerfen, C.
(2013) When gender and looking go hand in hand: Grammatical gender processing in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(2), 353–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fayol, M., Largy, P., & Lemaire, P.
(1994) Cognitive overload and orthographic errors: When cognitive overload enhances subject–verb agreement errors. A study in French written language. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 47(2), 437–464. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M.
(1999) A rose by any other name: Long-term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 469–495. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V. S., & Pashler, H.
(2002) Central bottleneck influences on the processing stages of word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(6), 1187–1199. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferretti, T. R., McRae, K., & Hatherell, A.
(2001) Integrating verbs, situation schemas, and thematic role concepts. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(4), 516–547. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., & Frenck-Mestre, C.
(2011) Grammatical gender processing in L2: Electrophysiological evidence of the effect of L1–L2 syntactic similarity. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 379–399. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., Martin, C. D., Moreno, E. M., & Costa, A.
(2014) Can bilinguals see it coming? Word anticipation in L2 sentence reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(5), 1461–1469. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., Ruiz-Tada, E., & Costa, A.
(2015) How do you know I was about to say “book”? Anticipation processes affect speech processing and lexical recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(6), 768–780. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2016) Anticipation processes in L2 speech comprehension: Evidence from ERPs and lexical recognition task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 213–219. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gambi, C., Pickering, M. J., & Rabagliati, H.
(2016) Beyond associations: Sensitivity to structure in pre-schoolers’ linguistic predictions. Cognition, 157, 340–351. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J.
(2007) Automaticity of language production in monologue and dialogue. In A. S. Meyer, L. R. Wheeldon, & A. Krott (Eds.), Automaticity and control in language processing (pp. 1–20). Psychology Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grossi, G.
(2006) Relatedness proportion effects on masked associative priming: An ERP study. Psychophysiology, 43(1), 21–30. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T., Lau, E., & Ling, W.
(2020) How classifiers facilitate predictive processing in L1 and L2 Chinese: The role of semantic and grammatical cues. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(2), 221–234. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Barkhuysen, P. N.
(2006) Language production and working memory: The case of subject-verb agreement. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(1–3), 181–204. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Moors, A.
(2017) On the automaticity of language processing. In H.-J. Schmid (Ed.), Entrenchment and the psychology of language learning: How we reorganize and adapt linguistic knowledge (pp. 201–225). American Psychological Association. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H.
(2010) Ultimate attainment in L2 inflection: Performance similarities between non-native and native speakers. Lingua, 120(4), 901–931. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Grammatical gender in adult L2 acquisition: Relations between lexical and syntactic variability. Second Language Research, 29(1), 33–56. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2015) Semantics and morphosyntax in predictive L2 sentence processing. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 53(3), 277–306. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2016) Learning (not) to predict: Grammatical gender processing in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 32(2), 277–307. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2018) The bilingual mental lexicon in L2 sentence processing. Second Language, 17, 5–27. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoversten, L. J., & Traxler, M. J.
(2020) Zooming in on zooming out: Partial selectivity and dynamic tuning of bilingual language control during reading. Cognition, 195, 104118. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F.
(2015) Four central questions about prediction in language processing. Brain Research, 1626, 118–135. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Guerra, E.
(2019) Effects of speech rate, preview time of visual context, and participant instructions reveal strong limits on prediction in language processing. Brain Research, 1706, 196–208. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Janse, E.
(2016) Individual differences in working memory and processing speed predict anticipatory spoken language processing in the visual world. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 80–93. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J.
(2002) Towards a unified account of the representation, processing and acquisition of second language knowledge. Second Language Research, 18(3), 193–223. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Corley, M., & Pickering, M. J.
(2018) A cognitive load delays predictive eye movements similarly during L1 and L2 comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(2), 251–264. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Corley, M., Pickering, M. J., Martin, A. E., & Nieuwland, M. S.
(2016) Predicting form and meaning: Evidence from brain potentials. Journal of Memory and Language, 86, 157–171. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Martin, A. E., & Nieuwland, M. S.
(2017a) On predicting form and meaning in a second language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(4), 635–652. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2017b) How robust are prediction effects in language comprehension? Failure to replicate article-elicited N400 effects. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 32(8), 954–965. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Nguyen, H. T. T., & Knoeferle, P.
(2021) Anticipatory use of verb and classifier constraints in Vietnamese-German bilinguals. Manuscript in preparationGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Pickering, M. J., & Corley, M.
(2018) Investigating the time-course of phonological prediction in native and non-native speakers of English: A visual world eye-tracking study. Journal of Memory and Language, 98, 1–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ivanova, I., & Costa, A.
(2008) Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production? Acta Psychologica, 127(2), 277–288. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaan, E.
(2014) Predictive sentence processing in L2 and L1: What is different? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4(2), 257–282. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. L.
(2003) The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 133–156. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kukona, A., Braze, D., Johns, C. L., Mencl, W. E., Van Dyke, J. A., Magnuson, J. S., Pugh, K. R., Shankweiler, D. P., & Tabor, W.
(2016) The real-time prediction and inhibition of linguistic outcomes: Effects of language and literacy skill. Acta Psychologica, 171, 72–84. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kukona, A., Fang, S. Y., Aicher, K. A., Chen, H., & Magnuson, J. S.
(2011) The time course of anticipatory constraint integration. Cognition, 119(1), 23–42. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Laszlo, S., & Federmeier, K. D.
(2009) A beautiful day in the neighborhood: An event-related potential study of lexical relationships and prediction in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 61(3), 326–338. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lau, E. F., Holcomb, P. J., & Kuperberg, G. R.
(2013) Dissociating N400 effects of prediction from association in single-word contexts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(3), 484–502. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M.
(1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S.
(1999) A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1–75. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A.
(2010) Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(4), 447–464. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martin, C. D., Thierry, G., Kuipers, J. R., Boutonnet, B., Foucart, A., & Costa, A.
(2013) Bilinguals reading in their second language do not predict upcoming words as native readers do. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(4), 574–588. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mitsugi, S., & MacWhinney, B.
(2016) The use of case marking for predictive processing in second language Japanese. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 19–35. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Moors, A.
(2016) Automaticity: Componential, causal, and mechanistic explanations. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 263–287. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Neely, J. H.
(1991) Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nicenboim, B., Vasishth, S., & Rösler, F.
(2020) Are words pre-activated probabilistically during sentence comprehension? Evidence from new data and a bayesian random-effects meta-analysis using publicly available data. Neuropsychologia, 142, 107427. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nieuwland, M. S., Politzer-Ahles, S., Heyselaar, E., Segaert, K., Darley, E., Kazanina, N., Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn, S., Bartolozzi, F., Kogan, V., Ito, A., Mézière, D., Barr, D. J., Rousselet, G. a, Ferguson, H. J., Busch-Moreno, S., Fu, X., Tuomainen, J., Kulakova, E., Husband, E. M., … Huettig, F.
(2018) Large-scale replication study reveals a limit on probabilistic prediction in language comprehension. ELife, 7, e33468. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peters, R. E., Grüter, T., & Borovsky, A.
(2018) Vocabulary size and native speaker self-identification influence flexibility in linguistic prediction among adult bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(6), 1439–1469. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Gambi, C.
(2018) Predicting while comprehending language: A theory and review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(10), 1002–1044. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S.
(2007) Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(3), 105–110. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 329–392. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G.
(1981) Does activation really spread? Psychological Review, 88(5), 454–462. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roelofs, A., & Piai, V.
(2011) Attention demands of spoken word planning: A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–14. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, N., & Hulstijn, J. H.
(2009) Automaticity in bilingualism and second language learning. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 371–388). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W.
(1977) Controlled and automatic human information processing, II: Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127–190. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M. J., & Marian, V.
(1999) Cross talk between native and second languages: Partial activation of an irrelevant lexicon. Psychological Science, 10(3), 281–284. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Bergen, G., & Flecken, M.
(2017) Putting things in new places: Linguistic experience modulates the predictive power of placement verb semantics. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 26–42. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Petten, C., & Luka, B. J.
(2012) Prediction during language comprehension: Benefits, costs, and ERP components. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83(2), 176–190. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wicha, N. Y. Y., Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M.
(2004) Anticipating words and their gender: An event-related brain potential study of semantic integration, gender expectancy, and gender agreement in Spanish sentence reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(7), 1272–1288. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wolpert, D. M.
(1997) Computational approaches to motor control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(6), 209–216. CrossrefGoogle Scholar