Part of
Prediction in Second Language Processing and Learning
Edited by Edith Kaan and Theres Grüter
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 12] 2021
► pp. 4768
References
Bader, M., & Häussler, J.
(2010) Word order in German: A corpus study. Lingua, 120(3), 717–762. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J.
(2008) Analyzing ‘visual world’ eyetracking data using multilevel logistic regression. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 457–474. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Gann, T. M., & Pierce, R. S.
(2011) Anticipatory baseline effects and information integration in visual world studies. Acta Psychologica, 137(2), 201–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J.
(2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S.
(2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P.
(2001) PRAAT, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International, 5(9/10), 341–345.Google Scholar
Bornkessel, I., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D.
(2002) Grammar overrides frequency: Evidence from the online processing of flexible word order. Cognition, 85(2), B21–B30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chambers, C. G., & Cooke, H.
(2009) Lexical competition during second-language listening: Sentence context, but not proficiency, constrains interference from the native lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(4), 1029–1040. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C.
(2006) Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(1), 3–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Some notes on the Shallow Structure Hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(3), 693–706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cunnings, I.
(2017) Parsing and working memory in bilingual sentence processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(4), 659–678. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkgraaf, A., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Duyck, W.
(2017) Predicting upcoming information in native-language and non-native-language auditory word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(5), 917–930. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019) Prediction and integration of semantics during L2 and L1 listening. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(7), 881–900. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dink, J. W., & Ferguson, B.
(2015) eyetrackingR: An R library for eye-tracking data analysis. Retrieved from [URL]
Donnelly, S., & Verkuilen, J.
(2017) Empirical logit analysis is not logistic regression. Journal of Memory and Language, 94, 28–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Crepaldi, D., Meyer, A. S., New, B., Pliatsikas, C., Smolka, E., & Brysbaert, M.
(2018) Multipic: A standardized set of 750 drawings with norms for six European languages. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(4), 808–816. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Valdés Kroff, J. R., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., & Gerfen, C.
(2013) When gender and looking go hand in hand: Grammatical gender processing in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(2), 353–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C., Kim, S. K., Choo, H., Ghio, A., Herschensohn, J., & Koh, S.
(2019) Look and listen! The online processing of Korean case by native and non-native speakers. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(3), 385–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerth, S., Otto, C., Felser, C., & Nam, Y.
(2017) Strength of garden-path effects in native and non-native speakers’ processing of object–subject ambiguities. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(2), 125–144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T., Lau, E., & Ling, W.
(2020) How classifiers facilitate predictive processing in L1 and L2 Chinese: The role of semantic and grammatical cues. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(2), 221–234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Häussler, J., & Bader, M.
(2011) Grammar- versus frequency-driven syntactic ambiguity resolution: The case of double-object constructions. In M. Lamers & P. de Swart (Eds.), Case, word order and prominence: Interacting cues in language production and comprehension (pp. 273–301). Springer.Google Scholar
Hopp, H.
(2015) Semantics and morphosyntax in predictive L2 sentence processing. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 53(3), 277–306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) The bilingual mental lexicon in L2 sentence processing. Second Language, 17, 5–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H., & Lemmerth, N.
(2018) Lexical and syntactic congruency in L2 predictive gender processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(1), 171–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F.
(2015) Four central questions about prediction in language processing. Brain Research, 1626, 118–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Corley, M., & Pickering, M. J.
(2018) A cognitive load delays predictive eye movements similarly during L1 and L2 comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(2), 251–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Pickering, M. J., & Corley, M.
(2018) Investigating the time-course of phonological prediction in native and non-native speakers of English: A visual world eye-tracking study. Journal of Memory and Language, 98, 1–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. L.
(2003) The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 133–156. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B.
(2017) lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matin, E., Shao, K. C., & Boff, K. R.
(1993) Saccadic overhead: Information-processing time with and without saccades. Perception & Psychophysics, 53(4), 372–380. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matuschek, H., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., Baayen, H., & Bates, D.
(2017) Balancing Type I error and power in linear mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 94, 305–315. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mirman, D., Dixon, J. A., & Magnuson, J. S.
(2008) Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 475–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitrofanova, N., Sekerina, I. A., Sauermann, A., Gagarina, N., & Westergaard, M.
(2019, June 27). Cross-linguistic differences in predictive use of grammatical case in Russian and German (comparing monolingual and bilingual children) [Conference presentation]. International Symposium on Bilingualism (ISB), University of Alberta, Canada.
Mitsugi, S., & MacWhinney, B.
(2016) The use of case marking for predictive processing in second language Japanese. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 19–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peters, R. E., Grüter, T., & Borovsky, A.
(2015) Anticipatory and locally coherent lexical activation varies as a function of language proficiency. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A. S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. D. Jennings, & P. P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1865–1870). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Pozzan, L., & Trueswell, J.
(2016) Second language processing and revision of garden-path sentences: A visual world study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(3), 636–643. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team
(2020) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from [URL]
Schlenter, J.
(2019) Predictive language processing in late bilinguals: Evidence from visual-world eye-tracking (Doctoral dissertation). University of Potsdam. publish.UP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Bergen, G., & Flecken, M.
(2017) Putting things in new places: Linguistic experience modulates the predictive power of placement verb semantics. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 26–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Karaca, Figen, Susanne Brouwer, Sharon Unsworth & Falk Huettig
2024. Morphosyntactic predictive processing in adult heritage speakers: effects of cue availability and spoken and written language experience. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 39:1  pp. 118 ff. DOI logo
Parshina, Olga, Anastasiya Lopukhina & Irina A. Sekerina
2022. Can Heritage Speakers Predict Lexical and Morphosyntactic Information in Reading?. Languages 7:1  pp. 60 ff. DOI logo
Pérez, A. I., E. Schmidt & I. M. Tsimpli
2023. Inferential evaluation and revision in L1 and L2 text comprehension: An eye movement study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Schlenter, Judith
2023. Prediction in bilingual sentence processing: How prediction differs in a later learned language from a first language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 26:2  pp. 253 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 20 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.