Chapter published in:
Prediction in Second Language Processing and Learning
Edited by Edith Kaan and Theres Grüter
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 12] 2021
► pp. 116137
References

References

Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y.
(1999) Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73, 247–264. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G. T. M., & Mirković, J.
(2009) Incrementality and prediction in human sentence processing. Cognitive Science, 33(4), 583–609. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L., & Fernald, A.
(2012) Knowing a lot for one’s age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(4), 417–436. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brouwer, S., Özkan, D., & Küntay, A. C.
(2017) Semantic prediction in monolingual and bilingual children. In E. Blom, L. Cornips, & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), Cross-linguistic influence in bilingualism, (pp. 48–73). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brouwer, S., Özkan, D., & Küntay, A. C.
(2019) Verb-based prediction during language processing: The case of Dutch and Turkish. Journal of Child Language, 46(1), 80–97. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brouwer, S., Sprenger, S., & Unsworth, S.
(2017) Processing grammatical gender in Dutch: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 50–65. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Candan, A., Küntay, A. C., Yeh, Y., Cheung, H., Wagner, L., & Naigles, L. R.
(2012) Language and age effects in children’s processing of word order. Cognitive Development, 27(3), 205–221. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Choi, Y., & Trueswell, J. C.
(2010) Children’s (in)ability to recover from garden paths in a verb-final language: Evidence for developing control in sentence processing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(1), 41–61. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, R. M.
(1974) The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language. Cognitive Psychology, 6(1), 84–107. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., & Chang, F.
(2014) The P-chain: Relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634), 20120394. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dijkgraaf, A., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Duyck, W.
(2017) Predicting upcoming information in native-language and non-native language auditory word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(5), 917–930. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2019) Prediction and integration of semantics during L2 and L1 listening. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(7), 881–900. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Valdés Kroff, J. R., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., & Gerfen, C.
(2013) When gender and looking go hand in hand. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(2), 353–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Federmeier, K. D.
(2007) Thinking ahead: The role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 44(4), 491–505. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M.
(2005) Aging in context: Age-related changes in context use during language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 42(2), 133–141. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, F., & Chantavarin, S.
(2018) Integration and prediction in language processing: A synthesis of old and new. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 443–448. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A.
(2015) Prediction is a question of experience. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 5(4), 465–469. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., Martin, C. D., Moreno, E. M., & Costa, A.
(2014) Can bilinguals see it coming? Word anticipation in L2 sentence reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(5), 1461–1469. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gambi, C., Gorrie, F., Pickering, M. J., & Rabagliati, H.
(2018) The development of linguistic prediction: Predictions of sound and meaning in 2- to 5-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 173, 351–370. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gambi, C., Jindal, P., Sharpe, S., Pickering, M. J., & Rabagliati, H.
(2020) The relation between preschoolers’ vocabulary development and their ability to predict and recognize words. Child Development. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gambi, C., Pickering, M. J., & Rabagliati, H.
(2016) Beyond associations: Sensitivity to structure in pre-schoolers’ linguistic predictions. Cognition, 157, 340–351. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., Paradis, J., & Crago, M.
(2004) Dual language development and disorders: A handbook on bilingualism and second language learning. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Gibson, E., Bergen, L., & Piantadosi, S. T.
(2013) The rational integration of noisy evidence and prior semantic expectations in sentence interpretation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(20), 8051–8056. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T., Lau, E., & Ling, W.
(2018) L2 listeners rely on the semantics of classifiers to predict. In A. B. Bertolini & M. J. Kaplan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 303–316). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Grüter, T., Rohde, H., & Schafer, A. J.
(2014) The role of discourse-level expectations in non-native speakers’ referential choices. In W. Orman & M. J. Valleau (Eds.), Proceedings of the 38th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 179–191). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
(2017) Coreference and discourse coherence in L2: The roles of grammatical aspect and referential form. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(2), 199–229. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hale, J.
(2001) A probabilistic Earley parser as a psycholinguistic model. In Proceedings of the second meeting of North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics on Technologies (pp. 159–166). Stroudsburg, PA: Association for Computational Linguistics. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Havron, N., de Carvalho, A., Fiévet, A. C., & Christophe, A.
(2019) Three-to four-year-old children rapidly adapt their predictions and use them to learn novel word meanings. Child Development, 90(1), 82–90. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hickok, G.
(2012) Computational neuroanatomy of speech production. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 135–145. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., & Shanks, K. F.
(2020) The quality of child-directed speech depends on the speaker’s language proficiency. Journal of Child Language, 47, 132–145. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H.
(2013) Grammatical gender in adult L2 acquisition: Relations between lexical and syntactic variability. Second Language Research, 29(1), 33–56. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2015) Semantics and morphosyntax in L2 predictive sentence processing. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 53(3), 277–306. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H. & Lemmerth, N.
(2018) Lexical and syntactic congruency in L2 predictive gender processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(1), 171–199. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F.
(2015) Four central questions about prediction in language processing. Brain Research, 1626, 118–135. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Brouwer, S.
(2015) Delayed anticipatory spoken language processing in adults with dyslexia – Evidence from eye-tracking. Dyslexia, 21(2), 97–122. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Janse, E.
(2016) Individual differences in working memory and processing speed predict anticipatory spoken language processing in the visual world. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 80–93. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Mani, N.
(2016) Is prediction necessary to understand language? Probably not. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 19–31. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Pickering, M. J.
(2019) Literacy advantages beyond reading: Prediction of spoken language. Trends in Cognitive Science, 23(6), 464–475. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A.
(2014) Relative language exposure, processing efficiency and vocabulary in Spanish–English bilingual toddlers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 189–202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Corley, M., & Pickering, M. J.
(2018) A cognitive load delays predictive eye movements similarly during L1 and L2 comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(2), 251–264. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E. K.
(2005) Grammatical gender and early word recognition in Dutch. In A. Brugos, M. R. Clark-Cotton, & S. Ha (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 320–30). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Kaan, E.
(2014) Predictive sentence processing in L2 and L1: What is different? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4(2), 257–282. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kamide, Y., Scheepers, C., & Altmann, G. T.
(2003) Integration of syntactic and semantic information in predictive processing: Cross-linguistic evidence from German and English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32, 37–55 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kochari, A. R. & Flecken, M.
(2019) Lexical prediction in language comprehension: a replication study of grammatical gender effects in Dutch. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(2), 239–253. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kootstra, G. J., & Doedens, W. J.
(2016) How multiple sources of experience influence bilingual syntactic choice: Immediate and cumulative cross-language effects of structural priming, verb bias, and language dominance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(4), 710–732. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kukona, A., Braze, D., Johns, C. L., Mencl, W. E., Van Dyke, J. A., Magnuson, J. S., Pugh, K. R., Shankweiler, D. P., Tabor, W.
(2016) The real-time prediction and inhibition of linguistic outcomes: Effects of language and literacy skill. Acta Psychologica, 171, 72–84. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, G. R., & Jaeger, T. F.
(2016) What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 32–59. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lemmerth, N. & Hopp, H.
(2019) Gender processing in simultaneous and successive bilingual children: Cross-linguistic lexical and syntactic influences. Language Acquisition, 26(1), 21–45. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levy, R.
(2008) Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106(3), 1126–1177. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A.
(2007) Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 18(3), 193–198. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lijewska, A.
(2020) Cognate processing effects in bilingual lexical access. In R. Heredia & A. Cieślicka (Eds.), Bilingual lexical ambiguity resolution (pp. 71–95). Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605–621. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lukyanenko, C., & Fisher, C.
(2016) Where are the cookies? Two-and three-year-olds use number-marked verbs to anticipate upcoming nouns. Cognition, 146, 349–370. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mani, N., Daum, M. M., & Huettig, F.
(2016) “Pro-active” in many ways: Developmental evidence for a dynamic pluralistic approach to prediction. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(11), 2189–2201. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mani, N., & Huettig, F.
(2012) Prediction during language processing is a piece of cake – but only for skilled producers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(4), 843–847. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) Word reading skill predicts anticipation of upcoming spoken language input: A study of children developing proficiency in reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 264–279. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martin, C. D., Thierry, G., Kuipers, J. R., Boutonnet, B., Foucart, A., & Costa, A.
(2013) Bilinguals reading in their second language do not predict upcoming words as native readers do. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(4), 574–588. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, B.
(1978) Second-language acquisition in childhood. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Meir, N., Parshina, O., & Sekerina, I. A.
(2020) The interaction of morphological cues in bilingual sentence processing: An eye-tracking study. In M. Brown & A. Kohut (Eds.), Proceedings of the 44th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 376–389). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Melançon, A., & Shi, R.
(2015) Representations of abstract grammatical feature agreement in young children. Journal of Child Language, 42(6), 1379–1393. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meroni, L., Smeets, L. & Unsworth, S.
(2017) Cross-linguistic influence in scope ambiguity: Evidence for acceleration. In E. Blom, L. Cornips, & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), Cross-linguistic influence in bilingualism: In honor of Aafke Hulk (pp. 181–206). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mishra, R. K., Singh, N., Pandey, A., & Huettig, F.
(2012) Spoken language-mediated anticipatory eye movements are modulated by reading ability: Evidence from Indian low and high literates. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5(1). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mitsugi, S., & MacWhinney, B.
(2016) The use of case marking for predictive processing in second language Japanese. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 19–35. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nieuwland, M. S., Politzer-Ahles, S., Heyselaar, E., Segaert, K., Darley, E., Kazanina, N., Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn, S., Bartolozzi, F., Kogan, V., Ito, A., Mézière, D., Barr, D. J., Rousselet, G. a, Ferguson, H. J., Busch-Moreno, S., Fu, X., Tuomainen, J., Kulakova, E., Husband, E. M., … Huettig, F.
(2018) Large-scale replication study reveals a limit on probabilistic prediction in language comprehension. ELife, 7, e33468. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Norris, D., McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A.
(2016) Prediction, Bayesian inference and feedback in speech recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 4–18. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Özge, D., Küntay, A., & Snedeker, J.
(2019) Why wait for the verb? Turkish speaking children use case markers for incremental language comprehension. Cognition, 183, 152–180. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J.
(2011) Individual differences in child English second language acquisition: Comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(3), 213–237. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Gambi, C.
(2018) Predicting while comprehending language: A theory and review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(10), 1002–1044. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S.
(2013) An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 329–347. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roesch, A. D., & Chondrogianni, V.
(2016) “Which mouse kissed the frog?” Effects of age of onset, length of exposure, and knowledge of case marking on the comprehension of wh-questions in German-speaking simultaneous and early sequential bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 43(3), 635–661. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F.
(2015) Verbal and nonverbal predictors of language-mediated anticipatory eye movements. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 720–730. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2005) Syntactic optionality in language development. In L. Cornips & K. Corrigan (Eds.), Syntax and variation: Reconciling the biological and the social (pp. 55–80). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M., Eberhard, K., & Sedivy, J.
(1995) Integration of visual and linguistic information during spoken language comprehension. Science, 268(5217), 1632–1634. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S.
(2013a) Assessing age of onset effects in (early) child L2 acquisition. Language Acquisition, 20(2), 74–92. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013b) Assessing the role of current and CUMULATIVE exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(1), 86–110. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2016) Quantity and quality of language input in bilingual language development. In E. Nicoladis & S. Montanari (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan (pp. 103–122). Mouton de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S., Brouwer, S., de Bree, E., & Verhagen, J.
(2019) Predicting bilingual preschoolers’ patterns of language development: Degree of non-native input matters. Applied Psycholinguistics, 40(5), 1189–1219. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Bergen, G., & Flecken, M.
(2017) Putting things in new places: Linguistic experience modulates the predictive power of placement verb semantics. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 26–42. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Dijk, C. N., Dijkstra, T., & Unsworth, S.
under review). Cross-linguistic influence during online sentence processing in bilingual children.
Van Dijk, C. N., van Wonderen, E., Koutamanis, E., Kootstra, G. J., Dijkstra, T., & Unsworth, S.
(in press) Cross-linguistic influence in simultaneous bilingual children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Child Language. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Gompel, R., & Arai, M.
(2018) Structural priming in bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(3), 448–455. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van Heugten, M., & Shi, R.
(2009) French-learning toddlers use gender information on determiners during word recognition. Developmental Science, 12(3), 419–425. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Petten, C., & Luka, B. J.
(2012) Prediction during language comprehension: Benefits, costs, and ERP components. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83(2), 176–190. CrossrefGoogle Scholar