Part of
Cross-language Influences in Bilingual Processing and Second Language Acquisition
Edited by Irina Elgort, Anna Siyanova-Chanturia and Marc Brysbaert
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 16] 2023
► pp. 102125
References
Adelman, J. S., Johnson, R. L., McCormick, S. F., McKague, M., Kinoshita, S., Bowers, J. S., Perry, J. R., Lupker, S. J., Forster, K. I., Cortese, M. J., Scaltritti, M., Aschenbrenner, A. J., Coane, J. H., White, L., Yap, M. J., Davis, C., Kim, J., & Davis, C. J.
(2014) A behavioral database for masked form priming. Behavior Research Methods, 46(4), 1052–1067. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andrews, S.
(1996) Lexical retrieval and selection processes: Effects of transposed-letter confusability. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(6), 775–800. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997) The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(4), 439–461. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Cortese, M. J., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., Neely, J. H., Nelson, D. L., Simpson, G. B., & Treiman, R.
(2007) The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bijeljac-Babic, R., Biardeau, A., & Grainger, J.
(1997) Masked orthographic priming in bilingual word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 25(4), 447–457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boddaert, G., Cornut, C., & Casalis, S.
(2021) Integration of newly learned L2 words into the mental lexicon is modulated by vocabulary learning method. Acta Psychologica, 212, 103220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., & Mandera, P.
(2021) Which words do English non-native speakers know? New supernational levels based on yes/no decision. Second Language Research, 37(2), 207–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., Lagrou, E., & Stevens, M.
(2017) Visual word recognition in a second language: A test of the lexical entrenchment hypothesis with lexical decision times. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(3), 530–548. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers, E.
(2018) The word frequency effect in word processing: An updated review. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(1), 45–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers, E.
(2016) How many words do we know? Practical estimates of vocabulary size dependent on word definition, the degree of language input and the participant’s age. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casaponsa, A., Thierry, G., & Duñabeitia, J. A.
(2020) The role of orthotactics in language switching: An ERP investigation using masked language priming. Brain Sciences, 10(1), 22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christoffels, I. K., de Groot, A. M. B., & Kroll, J. F.
(2006) Memory and language skills in simultaneous interpreters: The role of expertise and language proficiency. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(3), 324–345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J.
(2001) DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Commissaire, E., Audusseau, J., & Casalis, S.
(2019) Disentangling cross-language orthographic neighborhood from markedness effects in L2 visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(1), 353–359. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davis, C. J.
(2010) The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological Review, 117(3), 713–758. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, K., Lemhöfer, K., & Brysbaert, M.
(2013) The word frequency effect in first- and second-language word recognition: a lexical entrenchment account. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), 66(5), 843–863. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, K., Ziegler, J. C., & Grainger, J.
(2010) Fast phonology and the bimodal interactive activation model. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(5), 764–778. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., Hilberink-Schulpen, B., & van Heuven, W. J. B.
(2010a) Repetition and masked form priming within and between languages using word and nonword neighbors. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(3), 341–357. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., Miwa, K., Brummelhuis, B., Sappelli, M., & Baayen, H.
(2010b) How cross-language similarity and task demands affect cognate recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 62(3), 284–301. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., van Geffen, A., Hieselaar, W., & Peeters, D.
(2022a) Multilink+: A computational model for printed word retrieval in different languages, participant groups, and tasks (Manuscript submitted for publication). Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University.
Dijkstra, T., Peeters, D., Hieselaar, W., & van Geffen, A.
(2022b) Orthographic and semantic priming effects in neighbour cognates: Experiments and simulations. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1–13. DOI logo
Dijkstra, T., & van Heuven, W. J. B.
(1998) The BIA model and bilingual word recognition. In Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition (pp. 189–225). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2002) The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5(3), 175–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., van Heuven, W. J. B., & Grainger, J.
(1998) Simulating cross-language competition with the bilingual interactive activation model. Psychologica Belgica, 38(3–4), 177–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & van Heuven, W. J. B.
(2018) Visual word recognition in multilinguals. In S. -A. Rueschemeyer & M. G. Gaskell (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T., Wahl, A., Buytenhuijs, F., van Halem, N., Al-Jibouri, Z., de Korte, M., & Rekké, S.
(2019) Multilink: A computational model for bilingual word recognition and word translation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(4), 657–679. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dirix, N., Cop, U., Drieghe, D., & Duyck, W.
(2017) Cross-lingual neighborhood effects in generalized lexical decision and natural reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(6), 887–915. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dufau, S., Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C.
(2012) How to say “no” to a nonword: A leaky competing accumulator model of lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(4), 1117–1128. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duyck, W., Vanderelst, D., Desmet, T., & Hartsuiker, R. J.
(2008) The frequency effect in second-language visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(4), 850–855. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferrand, L.
(2001) Cognition et lecture: Processus de base de la reconnaissance des mots écrits chez l’adulte. De Boeck Supérieur. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., & Dijkstra, T.
(1992) On the representation and use of language information in bilinguals. In Advances in Psychology 83 (pp. 207–220). Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M.
(1996) Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103(3), 518–565. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., Midgley, K., & Holcomb, P. J.
(2010) Re-thinking the bilingual interactive-activation model from a developmental perspective (BIA-d). In M. Kail & M. Hickmann (Eds.), Language acquisition across linguistic and cognitive systems (pp. 267–283). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., & van Heuven, W. J. B.
(2003) Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception. In P. Bonin (Ed.), Mental lexicon: “Some words to talk about words” (pp. 1–24). Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Green, D. W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(02), 67–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsieh, M. C., Jeong, H., Dos Santos Kawata, K. H., Sasaki, Y., Lee, H. C., Yokoyama, S., Sugiura, M., & Kawashima, R.
(2017) Neural correlates of bilingual language control during interlingual homograph processing in a logogram writing system. Brain and Language, 174, 72–85. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsieh, M. -C., Jeong, H., Sugiura, M., & Kawashima, R.
(2021) Neural evidence of language membership control in bilingual word recognition: An fMRI study of cognate processing in Chinese–Japanese bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, A. M., & Grainger, J.
(1992) Testing a semistochastic variant of the interactive activation model in different word recognition experiments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 1174–1188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kerkhofs, R., Dijkstra, T., Chwilla, D. J., & de Bruijn, E. R.
(2006) Testing a model for bilingual semantic priming with interlingual homographs: RT and N400 effects. Brain Research, 1068(1), 170–183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keuleers, E., Lacey, P., Rastle, K., & Brysbaert, M.
(2012) The British Lexicon Project: Lexical decision data for 28,730 monosyllabic and disyllabic English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 287–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E.
(1994) Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(2), 149–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lemhöfer, K., Dijkstra, T., Schriefers, H., Baayen, R. H., Grainger, J., & Zwitserlood, P.
(2008) Native language influences on word recognition in a second language: A megastudy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(1), 12–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M.
(2019) Recognition times for 62 thousand English words: Data from the English Crowdsourcing Project. Behavior Research Methods, 52, 741–760. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Massaro, D. W.
(1988) Some criticisms of connectionist models of human performance. Journal of Memory and Language. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E.
(1981) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception. I: An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miwa, K., Dijkstra, T., Bolger, P., & Baayen, R. H.
(2014) Reading English with Japanese in mind: Effects of frequency, phonology, and meaning in different-script bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(3), 445–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mulder, K., van Heuven, W. J. B., & Dijkstra, T.
(2018) Revisiting the neighborhood: How L2 proficiency and neighborhood manipulation affect bilingual processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1860. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A.
(2004) The University of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(3), 402–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norris, D.
(2013) Models of visual word recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(10), 517–524. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Heuven, W. J. B.
(2000) Visual word recognition in monolingual and bilingual readers: Experiments and computational modeling (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Radboud University Nijmegen.
(2021) jIAM. Retrieved on 27 July 2022 from [URL]
(2005) Bilingual interactive activation models of word recognition in a second language. In V. Cook & B. Bassetti (Eds.), Second language writing systems (pp. 260–288). Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Heuven, W. J. B., & Coderre, E. L.
(2015) Orthographic processing in bilinguals. In J. W. Swieter (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing (pp. 308–326). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Heuven, W. J. B., Dijkstra, T., & Grainger, J.
(1998) Orthographic neighborhood effects in bilingual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(3), 458–483. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Heuven, W. J. B., Dijkstra, T., Grainger, J., & Schriefers, H.
(2001) Shared neighborhood effects in masked orthographic priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(1), 96–101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Heuven, W. J. B., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M.
(2014) SUBTLEX-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for British English. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(6), 1176–1190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Heuven, W. J. B., Schriefers, H., Dijkstra, T., & Hagoort, P.
(2008) Language conflict in the bilingual brain. Cerebral Cortex, 18(11), 2706–2716. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Kesteren, R., Dijkstra, T., & de Smedt, K.
(2012) Markedness effects in Norwegian-English bilinguals: Task-dependent use of language-specific letters and bigrams. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(11), 2129–2154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Wijnendaele, I., & Brysbaert, M.
(2002) Visual word recognition in bilinguals: Phonological priming from the second to the first language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(3), 616–627. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wen, Y., & van Heuven, W. J. B.
(2018) Limitations of translation activation in masked priming: Behavioural evidence from Chinese-English bilinguals and computational modelling. Journal of Memory and Language, 101, 84–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, J., Shu, H., McCandliss, B. D., & Zevin, J. D.
(2013) Orthographic influences on division of labor in learning to read Chinese and English: Insights from computational modeling. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(2), 354–366. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhao, X., & Li, P.
(2013) Simulating cross-language priming with a dynamic computational model of the lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(02), 288–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar