Part of
Cross-language Influences in Bilingual Processing and Second Language Acquisition
Edited by Irina Elgort, Anna Siyanova-Chanturia and Marc Brysbaert
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 16] 2023
► pp. 262293
References
Arai, M., Van Gompel, R. P. G., & Scheepers, C.
(2007) Priming ditransitive structures in comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 54, 218–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J.
(2007) Shared syntactic representations in bilinguals: Evidence for the role of word-order repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 931–949.Google Scholar
(2009) Persistence of emphasis in language production: A cross-linguistic approach. Cognition, 112, 300–317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) From language-specific to shared syntactic representations: The influence of second language proficiency on syntactic sharing in bilinguals. Cognition, 127, 287–306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, K.
(1986) Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boyd, J. K., & Goldberg, A. E.
(2011) Learning what not to say: The role of statistical preemption and categorization in A-adjective production. Language, 87, 55–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., McLean, J. F., & Stewart, A. J.
(2006) The role of local and global syntactic structure in language production: Evidence from syntactic priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 974–1010. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cai, Z. G., Pickering, M. J., Yan, H., & Branigan, H. P.
(2011) Lexical and syntactic representations in closely related languages: Evidence from Cantonese-Mandarin bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 431–445. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K.
(2006) Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113, 234–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Janciauskas, M., & Fitz, H.
(2012) Language adaptation and learning: Getting explicit about implicit learning. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6, 259–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Baumann, M., Pappert, S., & Fitz, H.
(2015) Do lemmas speak German? A verb position effect in German structural priming. Cognitive Science, 39, 1113–1130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, B., Jia, Y., Wang, Z., Dunlap, S., & Shin, J. -A.
(2013) Is word-order similarity necessary for cross-linguistic structural priming? Second Language Research, 29, 375–389. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Wang, S., & Hartsuiker, R. J.
(2022a) Do structure predictions persevere to multilinguals’ other languages? Evidence from cross-linguistic structural priming in comprehension [Submitted for publication]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2022b) Error-based structure prediction in language comprehension: evidence from verb bias effects in a visual-world structural priming paradigm for Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 48, 60–71.Google Scholar
de Bot, K.
(1992) A bilingual production model: Levelt’s “speaking” model approach. Applied Linguistics, 13, 1–24.Google Scholar
Desmet, T., & Declercq, M.
(2006) Cross-linguistic priming of syntactic hierarchical configuration information. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 610–632. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., & Cramer Scaltz, T. R.
(2008) Spanish-English L2 speakers’ use of subcategorization bias information in the resolution of temporary ambiguity during second language reading. Acta Psychologica, 128, 501–513. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Favier, S., Wright, A., Meyer, A., & Huettig, F.
(2019) Proficiency modulates between – but not within-language structural priming. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 3, 105–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fehér, O., Wonnacott, E., & Smith, K.
(2016) Structural priming in artificial languages and the regularisation of unpredictable variation. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 158–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitz, H., & Chang, F.
(2017) Meaningful questions: The acquisition of auxiliary inversion in a connectionist model of sentence production. Cognition, 166, 225–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H., & Liu, S.
(1999) Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 78–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fleischer, Z., Pickering, M. J., & McLean, J. F.
(2012) Shared information structure: Evidence from cross-linguistic priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 568–579. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flett, S., Branigan, H. P., & Pickering, M. J.
(2013) Are non-native structural preferences affected by native language preferences? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 751–760. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E., Piantadosi, S. T., Brink, K., Bergen, L., Lim, E., & Saxe, R.
(2013) A noisy-channel account of crosslinguistic word-order variation. Psychological Science, 24, 1079–1088. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E.
(2011) Corpus evidence of the viability of statistical preemption. Cognitive Linguistics, 22, 131–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Golestani, N., Alario, F. X., Meriaux, S., Le Bihan, D., Dehaene, S., & Pallier, C.
(2006) Syntax production in bilinguals. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1029–1040. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Beerts, S., Loncke, M., Desmet, T., & Bernolet, S.
(2016) Cross-linguistic structural priming in multilinguals: further evidence for shared syntax. Journal of Memory and Language, 90, 14–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Bernolet, S.
(2017) The development of shared syntax in second language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 219–234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Bernolet, S., Schoonbaert, S., Speybroeck, S., & Vanderelst, D.
(2008) Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 214–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E.
(2004) Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish/English bilinguals. Psychological Science, 15, 409–414. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Westenberg, C.
(2000) Word order priming in written and spoken sentence production. Cognition, 75, B27–B39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopman, E. W. M., & MacDonald, M. C.
(2018) Production practice during language learning improves comprehension. Psychological Science, 29, 961–971. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H., & Gruter, T.
(2021) The time-course of competition from the L1 grammar in L2 sentence processing: Evidence from cross-linguistic structural priming. Second Language Research. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Huang, J., Pickering, M. J., Chen, X., Cai, Z., Wang, S., & Branigan, H. P.
(2019) Does language similarity affect representational integration? Cognition, 185, 83–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hwang, H., Shin, J. -A., & Hartsuiker, R. J.
(2018) Late bilinguals share syntax unsparingly between L1 and L2: Evidence from crosslinguistically similar and different constructions. Language Learning, 68, 177–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacob, G., Katsika, K., Family, N., & Allen, S. E. M.
(2017) The role of constituent order and level of embedding in cross-linguistic structural priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 269–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Janciauskas, M., & Chang, F.
(2018) Input and age-dependent variation in second language learning: A connectionist account. Cognitive Science, 42, 519–554. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kamin, L. J.
(1969) Predictability, surprise, attention and conditioning. In B. A. Campbell & R. M Church (Eds.), Punishment and aversive behavior (pp. 279–296). Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Kantola, L., & Van Gompel, R. P. G.
(2011) Between- and within-language priming is the same: Evidence for shared bilingual syntactic representations. Memory & Cognition, 39, 276–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Khoe, Y. H., Tsoukala, C., Kootstra, G. J., & Frank, S. L.
(2021) Is structural priming between different languages a learning effect? Modelling priming as error-driven implicit learning. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience. Advance online publication. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., Tennant, E., & Nitschke, S.
(2015) Shared abstract representation of linguistic structure in bilingual sentence comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 1062–1067. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kim, Y. J., & McDonough, K.
(2008) The effect of interlocutor proficiency on the collaborative dialogue between Korean as a second language learners. Language Teaching Research, 12, 211–234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, E. K., Lu, D. H. Y., & Garnsey, S. M.
(2013) L1 word order and sensitivity to verb bias in L2 processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 761–775. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loebell, H., & Bock, K.
(2003) Structural priming across languages. Linguistics, 41, 791–824. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahowald, K., James, A., Futrell, R., & Gibson, E.
(2016) A meta-analysis of syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 5–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maier, R. M., Pickering, M. J., & Hartsuiker, R. J.
(2017) Does translation involve structural priming? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 1575–1589. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McDonough, K., & Fulga, A.
(2015) The detection and primed production of novel constructions. Language Learning, 65, 326–357. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montero-Melis, G., & Jaeger, T. F.
(2020) Changing expectations mediate adaptation in L2 production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23, 602–617. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muylle, M., Bernolet, S., & Hartsuiker, R. J.
(2020) The role of case marking and word order in cross-linguistic structural priming in late L2 acquisition. Language Learning, 70, 194–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021a) On the limits of shared syntactic representations: when word order variation blocks priming between an artificial language and Dutch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 47, 1471–1493.Google Scholar
(2021b) The development of shared syntactic representations in late L2-learners: Evidence from structural priming in an artificial language. Journal of Memory and Language, 119, 104233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nitschke, S., Kidd, E., & Serratrice, L.
(2010) First language transfer and long-term structural priming in comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 94–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P.
(1998) The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 633–651. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reitter, D., Keller, F., & Moore, J. D.
(2011) A computational cognitive model of syntactic priming. Cognitive Science, 35, 587–637. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Runnqvist, E., Gollan, T. H., Costa, A., & Ferreira, V. S.
(2013) A disadvantage in bilingual sentence production modulated by syntactic frequency and similarity across languages. Cognition, 129, 256–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salamoura, A., & Williams, J. N.
(2006) Lexical activation of cross-language syntactic priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 299–307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Processing verb argument structure across languages: Evidence for shared representations in the bilingual lexicon. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 627–660. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scheepers, C.
(2003) Syntactic priming of relative clause attachments: persistence of structural configuration in sentence production. Cognition, 89, 179–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schoonbaert, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J.
(2007) The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 153–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L.
Shin, J. A., & Christianson, K.
(2009) Syntactic processing in Korean-English bilingual production: Evidence from cross-linguistic structural priming. Cognition, 112, 175–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sijyeniyo, E., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Bernolet, S.
(2022) L2 proficiency modulates structural priming effects of dissimilar L2 syntactic structures: Evidence from the priming of Dutch passives in late French-Dutch bilinguals [Submitted for publication].Google Scholar
Thothathiri, M., & Snedeker, J.
(2008) Give and take: Syntactic priming during spoken language comprehension. Cognition, 108, 51–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tooley, K. M., Traxler, M. J., & Swaab, T. Y.
(2009) Electrophysiological and behavioral evidence of syntactic priming in sentence comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 25, 19–45.Google Scholar
Van Gompel, R. P. G., & Arai, M.
(2018) Structural priming in bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21, 448–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weber, K., & Indefrey, P.
(2009) Syntactic priming in German-English bilinguals during sentence comprehension. NeuroImage, 46, 1164–1172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weber, K., Christiansen, M. H., Indefrey, P., & Hagoort, P.
( 2019) Primed from the start: Syntactic priming during the first days of language learning. Language Learning, 69, 198–221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar