Chapter 4. Are cognate words “special”?
On the role of cognate words in language switching performance
One of the most remarkable abilities of bilingual speakers is that of keeping their two languages apart during speech production. Several researchers have argued that the attentional mechanisms responsible for this ability may vary depending on the L2-proficiency achieved by the bilinguals (Green, 1986; Costa & Santesteban, 2004). Here we use a language-switching task to further explore the extent to which these different attentional strategies depend on the cognate status of the words (Costa, Santesteban, & Caño, 2005). We also explore whether the production of cognates facilitates language-switching (Broersma & De Bot, 2006). Twenty-four low-proficient (L2-Learners) and twenty-four high-proficient (Bilinguals) Spanish-Catalan bilinguals performed a cued language-switching task including cognate and non-cognate words. Our results revealed the following: (a) responses to cognates were faster than to non-cognates; (b) the magnitude of the switching cost was similar regardless of the cognate status of either the preceding or the target word; (c) L2-Learners vs. Bilinguals showed different language switching cost patterns (replicating Costa & Santesteban, 2004). Overall, these findings suggest that the cognate status of words does not facilitate language switching and it does not alter the lexical selection mechanisms’ implicated during production.
References
Baus, C., Branzi, F., & Costa, A
(
2015)
On the mechanism and scope of language control in bilingual speech production. In
J.W. Schwieter (Ed.),
The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingual Processing (pp. 508-526). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.


Bobb, S., & Wodniecka, Z
(
2013)
Language switching in picture naming: What asymmetric switch costs (do not) tell us about inhibition in bilingual speech planning.
Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 568-585.


Baayen, R., Davidson, D., & Bates, D
(
2008)
Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items.
Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390-412.


Broersma, M
(
2009)
Triggered codeswitching between cognate languages.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 447-462.


Broersma, M., & De Bot, K
(
2006)
Triggered codeswitching: A corpus-based evaluation of the original triggering hypothesis and a new alternative.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 1-13.


Broersma, M., Isurin, L., Bultena, S., & De Bot, K
Bultena, S., Dijkstra, T., & Van Hell, J.G
(
2015a)
Language switch costs in sentence comprehension depend on language dominance: Evidence from self-paced reading.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 453–469.


Bultena, S., Dijkstra, T., & Van Hell, J.G
(
2015b)
Switch cost modulations in bilingual sentence processing: Evidence from shadowing.
Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 586-605.


Christoffels, I.K., Firk, C., & Schiller, N.O
(
2007)
Bilingual language control: An event-related brain potential study.
Brain Research, 1147, 192-208.


Clyne, M
(
1967)
Transference and triggering: Observations on the language assimilation of postwar German-speaking migrants in Australia. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.

Clyne, M
(
2003)
Dynamics of language contact. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.


Costa, A
(
2005)
Lexical access in bilingual production. In
J.F. Kroll &
A.M.B. De Groot (Eds.),
Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 308–325). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Costa, A., & Caramazza, A
(
1999)
Is lexical selection in bilingual speech production language-specific? Further evidence from Spanish-English and English-Spanish bilinguals.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 231-244.


Costa, A., Caramazza, A., & Sebastián-Gallés, N
(
2000)
The cognate facilitation effect: Implications for the models of lexical access.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1283-1296.


Costa, A., Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A
(
1999)
Lexical selection in bilinguals: Do words in the bilingual’s two lexicons compete for selection? Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 365–397.


Costa, A., & Santesteban, M
(
2004)
Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners.
Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 491-511.


Costa, A., Santesteban, M., & Caño, A
(
2005)
On the facilitatory effects of cognate words in bilingual speech production.
Brain & Language, 94, 94-103.


Costa, A., Santesteban, M., & Ivanova, I
(
2006)
How do high-proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? Inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both functional.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 32, 1057–1074.


De Bot, K., Broersma, M., & Isurin, L
(
2009)
Sources of triggering in codeswitching. In
Isurin, L.,
Winford, D. &
De Bot, K. (Eds.).
Cross-disciplinary approaches to codeswitching (pp. 85-102). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.


De Bleser, R., Dupont, P., Postler, J., Bormans, G., Speelman, D., Mortelmans, L., & Debrock, M
(
2003)
The organisation of the bilingual lexicon: A PET study.
Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16, 439-456.


De Groot, A.M.B., & Nas, G.L
(
1991)
Lexical representation of cognates and noncognates in compound bilinguals.
Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 90–123.


Fink, A., & Goldrick, M
(
2015)
Pervasive benefits of preparation in language switching.
Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22, 808-814.


Finkbeiner, M., Almeida, J., Janssen, N., & Caramazza, A
(
2006)
Lexical selection in bilingual speech production does not involve language suppression.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1075-1089.


Goldrick, M
(
2014)
Phonological processing: The retrieval and encoding of word form information in speech production. In
M. Goldrick,
V. Ferreira, &
M. Miozzo (Eds.)
The Oxford handbook of language production (pp. 228-244). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Gollan, T.H., & Ferreira, V.S
(
2009)
Should I stay or should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 640-665.


Green, D.W
(
1986)
Control, activation and resource.
Brain and Language, 27, 210–223.


Green, D.W
(
1998)
Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81.


Guasch, M., Boada, R., Ferré, P., & Sánchez-Casas, R
(
2013)
NIM: A Web-based Swiss Army knife to select stimuli for psycholinguistic studies.
Behavior Research Methods, 45, 765-771.


Jaeger, T.F
(
2008)
Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models.
Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434–446.


Kirsner, K., Lalor, E., & Hird, K
Kootstra, G.J., Van Hell, J.G., & Dijkstra, T
(
2012)
Priming of code-switches in sentences: The role of lexical repetition, cognates, and language proficiency.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 797–819.


Kroll, J.F., Bobb, S.C., Misra, M.M., & Guo, T
(
2008)
Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes.
Acta Psychologica, 128, 416-430.


Kroll, J.F., & Gollan, T.H
(
2014)
Speech planning in two languages: What bilinguals tell us about language production. In
M. Goldrick,
V. Ferreira, &
M. Miozzo (Eds.)
The Oxford handbook of language production (pp. 165-181). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Lotto, L., & De Groot, A.M.B
(
1998)
Effects of learning method and word type on acquiring vocabulary in an unfamiliar language.
Language Learning, 48, 31-69.


Martin, C.D., Strijkers, K., Santesteban, M., Escera, C., Hartsuiker, R.J., & Costa, A
(
2013)
The impact of early bilingualism on controlling a lately learnt language: An ERP study.
Frontiers in Psychology, 4: 815.


Meuter, R.F.I, & Allport, A
(
1999)
Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection.
Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 25-40.


Peeters, D., Dijkstra, T., & Grainger, J
(
2013)
The representation and processing of identical cognates by late bilinguals: RT and ERP effects.
Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 315–332.


Philipp, A.M., Gade, M., & Koch, I
(
2007)
Inhibitory processes in language switching? Evidence from switching language-defined response sets.
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 395-416.


Runnqvist, E., Strijkers, K., & Costa, A
(
2014)
Bilingual word access. In
M. Goldrick,
V. Ferreira, &
M. Miozzo (Eds.)
The Oxford handbook of language production (pp. 182-198). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Sánchez-Casas, R.M., & García-Albea, J.E
(
2005)
The representation of cognate and noncognate words in bilingual memory: Can cognate status be characterized as a special kind of morphological relation? In
J.F., Kroll &
A.M.B. De Groot (Eds.),
Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Sánchez-Casas, R.M., Davis, C.W., & García-Albea, J.E
(
1992)
Bilingual lexical processing: Exploring the cognate/non-cognate distinction.
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4, 293–310.


Strijkers, K., Costa, A., & Thierry, G
(
2010)
Tracking lexical access in speech production: electrophysiological correlates of word frequency and cognate effects.
Cerebral Cortex, 20, 912–928.


Tarłowski, A., Wodniecka, Z., & Marzecova, A
(
2013)
Language switching in the production of phrases.
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 42, 103-118.


Van Hell, J.G., & De Groot, A.M.B
(
1998)
Conceptual representation in bilingual memory: Effects of concreteness and cognate status in word association.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 193-211.


Verhoef, K., Roelofs, A., & Chwilla, D.J
(
2009)
Role of inhibition in language switching: Evidence from event-related brain potentials in overt picture naming.
Cognition, 110, 84-99.


Cited by
Cited by 3 other publications
Declerck, Mathieu
2020.
What about proactive language control?.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 27:1
► pp. 24 ff.

Santesteban, Mikel & John W. Schwieter
2020.
Lexical Selection and Competition in Bilinguals. In
Bilingual Lexical Ambiguity Resolution,
► pp. 126 ff.

[no author supplied]
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 march 2023. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.