References (43)
References
Allport, A., Styles, E., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta, & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV: Concious and nonconcious information processing (pp. 421-452). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Altmann, E. (2007). Cue-independent task-specific representations in task switching: Evidence from backward inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 892-899. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R., Davidson, D., & Bates, D. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390-412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R., & Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3, 12-28.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects model using Eigen and s4. R package version 1.1-7. <[URL]>Google Scholar
Baus, C., Branzi, F.M., Costa, A. (2015). On the mechanism and scope of language control in bilingual speech production. In J.W. Schwieter (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing (pp. 508-526). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bobb, S., & Wodniecka, Z. (2013). Language switching in picture naming. What asymmetric switch costs (do not) tell us about inhibition in bilingual speech planning. Special Issue of Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 568-585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Calabria, M., Hernández, M., Branzi, F., Costa, A., (2012) Qualitative differences between bilingual language control and executive control: evidence from task-switching. Frontiers in Psychology. 2, 1-10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cnaan, A., Laird, N., & Slasor, P. (1997). Using the general linear mixed model to analyse unbalanced repeated measures and longitudinal data. Statistics in Medicine, 16, 2349-2380. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M. (2004). Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 491-511. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Santesteban, M., & Ivanova, I. (2006). How do highly proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? Inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both functional. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 32, 1057-1074. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Bot, K., & Jaensch, C. (2015). What is special about L3 processing? Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 18, 130-144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, A., Grainger, J., & Van Heuven, W. (1999). Recognition of cognates and interlingual homographs: The neglected role of phonology. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 496-518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ecke, P. (2015). Parasitic vocabulary acquisition, cross-linguistic influence, and lexical retrieval in multilinguals. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 18, 145-162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Festman, J. (2008). Cross-language interference during trilingual picture naming in single and mixed language conditions. In M. Gibson, B. Hufeisen, & C. Personne (Hg.), Mehrsprachigkeit: lernen und lehren, Multilingualism: Learning and Instruction, Le Plurilinguisme: apprendre et enseigner, O Pluilinguismo: aprender e ensinar (pp. 109-119). Selected papers from the L3-Conference in Fribourg, Switzerland.Google Scholar
. (2009). Three languages in mind. Saarbrücken: VDM.Google Scholar
Festman, J., & Schwieter, J.W. (2015). Behavioural measures of language control: Production and comprehension. In J.W. Schwieter (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingual language processing (pp. 527-547). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fink, A., & Goldrick, M. (2015). Pervasive benefits of preparation in language switching. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22, 880-814. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67-81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guo, T., Liu, F., Chen, B., & Li., S. (2013). Inhibition of non-target languages in multilingual word production: Evidence from Uighur-Chinese-English trilinguals. Acta Psychologica, 143, 277-283. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guo, G., & Zhao, H. (2000). Multi-level modeling for binary data. Annual Review of Sociology, 26, 441-462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jost, K., De Baene, W., Koch, I., & Brass, M. (2013). A review of the role of cue processing in task switching. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 221, 5-14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kiesel, A., Steinhauer, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 849-874. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Linck, J., Schwieter, J.W., & Sunderman, G. (2012). Inhibitory control predicts language switching performance in trilingual speech production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 651-662. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Logan, G., & Bundesen, C. (2003). Clever homunculus: Is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 575-599. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 362-372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1423-1442. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2000). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234-249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meuter, R., & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 25-40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 134-140. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Monsell, S., Yeung, N., & Azuma, R. (2000). Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task? Psychological Research, 63, 250-264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mosca M., & Clahsen H. (2016). Examining language switching in bilinguals: The role of task-demand. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 415-424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, M. (2004). A neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Philipp, A., Gade, M., & Koch, I. (2007). Inhibitory processes in language switching: Evidence from switching language-defined response sets. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 395-416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Philipp, A., & Koch, I. (2009). Inhibition in language switching: What is inhibited when switching between languages in naming tasks? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1187-1195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Development Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. <[URL]>
Rogers, R., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207-231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rubinstein, J., Meyer, D., & Evans, J. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 763-797. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M., & Jarvis, S. (2014). Lexical access and lexical diversity in first language attrition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 17, 729-748. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Venables, W., & Ripley B. (2002). Modern Applied Statistics with S. Springer, New York, fourth edition. <[URL]> DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verhoef, K., Roelofs, A., & Chwilla, D. (2009). Role of inhibition in language switching: Evidence from event-related brain potentials in overt picture naming. Cognition, 110, 84-99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weissberger G., Wierenga C., Bondi M., Gollan T. (2012). Partially over-lapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology and Aging, 27, 959-974. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (7)

Cited by seven other publications

Palma, Pauline, Sarah Lee, Vegas Hodgins & Debra Titone
2023. From One Bilingual to the Next: An Iterated Learning Study on Language Evolution in Bilingual Societies. Cognitive Science 47:5 DOI logo
Shen, Qinfang
2023. Investigating the modulation of active preparation and passive dissipation on inhibitory control processes in the language switching paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology 14 DOI logo
Shen, Qinfang & Yixin Chen
2023. Investigating the modulation of stimulus types on language switching costs: Do semantic and repetition priming effect matter?. Frontiers in Psychology 14 DOI logo
Festman, Julia
2021. Learning and Processing Multiple Languages: The More the Easier?. Language Learning 71:S1  pp. 121 ff. DOI logo
Czapka, Sophia, Christiane Wotschack, Annegret Klassert & Julia Festman
2020. A path to the bilingual advantage: Pairwise matching of individuals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23:2  pp. 344 ff. DOI logo
Santesteban, Mikel & John W. Schwieter
2020. Lexical Selection and Competition in Bilinguals. In Bilingual Lexical Ambiguity Resolution,  pp. 126 ff. DOI logo
Mosca, Michela
2019. Trilinguals’ language switching: A strategic and flexible account. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 72:4  pp. 693 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.