Article published in:
Cognitive Control and Consequences of Multilingualism
Edited by John W. Schwieter
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 2] 2016
► pp. 351374
References

References

Akhtar, N., & Menjivar, J.
(2012) Cognitive and linguistic correlates of early exposure to more than one language. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 42, 41-78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, P.
(2002) Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8(2), 71-82. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Anton, E., Duñabeitia, J., Estevez, A., Hernandez, J., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L., … Carreiras, M.
(2014) Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5.Google Scholar
Baker, D., Eslinger, P., Benavides, M., Peters, E., Dieckmann, N., & Leon, J.
(2015) The cognitive impact of the education revolution: A possible cause of the Flynn Effect on population IQ. Intelligence, 49, 144-158. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D., & Sanchez, M.
(2014) The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 699-714. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bedore, L., Pena, E., Summers, C., Boerger, K., Resendiz, M., Greene, K., … Gillam, R.
(2012) The measure matters: Language dominance profiles across measures in Spanish-English bilingual children. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 15(3), 616-629. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Berguno, G., & Bowler, D.
(2004) Communicative interactions, knowledge of a second language, and theory of mind in young children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 165(3), 293-309. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Best, J., & Miller, P.
(2010) A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641-1660. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2010) Global-local and trail-making tasks by monolingual and bilingual children: Beyond inhibition. Developmental Psychology, 46(1), 93-105. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Barac, R.
(2012) Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122(1), 67-73. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Feng, X.
(2009) Language proficiency and executive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children and adults. Brain and Language, 109(2-3), 93-100. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M.
(2009) Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112(3), 494-500. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blackwell, K., Chatham, C., Wiseheart, M., & Munakata, Y.
(2014) A developmental window into trade-offs in executive function: The case of task switching versus response inhibition in 6-year-olds. Neuropsychologia, 62, 356-364. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blair, C., Granger, D., & Razza, R.
(2005) Cortisol reactivity is positively related to executive function in preschool children attending Head Start. Child Development, 76(3), 554-567. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bogulski, C., Rakoczy, M., Goodman, M., & Bialystok, E.
(2015) Executive control in fluent and lapsed bilinguals. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 18(3), 561-567. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Buac, M., & Kaushanskaya, M.
(2014) The relationship between linguistic and non-linguistic cognitive control skills in bilingual children from low socio-economic backgrounds. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E.
(2014) Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130(3), 278-288. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S., & Meltzoff, A.
(2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282-298. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Colombo, J.
(2001) The development of visual attention in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 337-367. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A.
(2013) Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135-168. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, V.
(2002) The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5(3), 175-97.Google Scholar
Duñabeitia, J., Hernández, J., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L., & Carreiras, M.
(2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 234-251. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Esposito, A., Baker-Ward, L., & Mueller, S.
(2013) Interference suppression vs. response inhibition: An explanation for the absence of a bilingual advantage in preschoolers’ Stroop Task performance. Cognitive Development, 28(4), 354-363. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Evans, G., & Kim, P.
(2013) Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 43-48. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Farah, M., Shera, D., Savage, J., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J., Brodsky, N., … Hurt, H.
(2006) Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Research, 1110, 166-174. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Filippi, R., Morris, J., Richardson, F., Bright, P., Thomas, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Marian, V.
(2015) Bilingual children show an advantage in controlling verbal interference during spoken language comprehension. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 18(3), 490-501. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, S., Scholnick, E., Bender, R., Vandergrift, N., Spieker, S., Hirsh Pasek, K.The NICHD Early Child Care Research Network
… (2014) Planning in middle childhood: Early predictors and later outcomes. Child Development, 85(4), 1446-1460. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Friesen, D., Latman, V., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E.
(2015) Attention during visual search: The benefit of bilingualism. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19(6), 693-702. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gandolfi, E., Viterbori, P., Traverso, L., & Usai, M.
(2014) Inhibitory processes in toddlers: A latent-variable approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 381. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garon, N., Bryson, S., & Smith, I.
(2008) Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31-60. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D., & Abutalebi, J.
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515-530. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F., & Li, P.
(2013) The Psycholinguistics of Bilingualism. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hackman, D., Gallop, R., Evans, G., & Farah, M.
(2015) Socioeconomic status and executive function: Developmental trajectories and mediation. Developmental Science, 18(5), 686-702. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hilchey, M., & Klein, R.M.
(2011) Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 625-658. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C.
(2013) Chapter 24 Executive function: Development, individual differences, and clinical insights. In J. Rakic (Ed.), Neural circuit development and function in the brain (pp. 429-445). Oxford, UK: Academic Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R.
(2005) Executive function and theory of mind in 2 year olds: A family affair? Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 645-668. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C., Ensor, R., Wilson, A., & Graham, A.
(2010) Tracking executive function across the transition to school: A latent variable approach. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(1), 20-36. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kalashnikova, M., & Mattock, K.
(2014) Maturation of executive functioning skills in early sequential bilingualism. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 17(1), 111-123. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kapa, L., & Colombo, J.
(2013) Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development, 28(3), 233-246. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, A., Kaufman, N.American Guidance Service
., & (Firm) (2004) KBIT2 Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.Google Scholar
Kovacs, A.
(2007)  Beyond Language: Childhood Bilingualism Enhances High-Level Cognitive Functions . In I. Kecskes & L. Albertazzi (Eds.), Cognitive aspects of bilingualism (pp. 301-324). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 497-514. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J., & Stewart, E.
(1994) Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(2), 149-174. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J., & Tokowicz, N.
(2005) Models of bilingual representation and processing. In Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 531-553). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kuipers, J.-R., & Thierry, G.
(2013) ERP-pupil size correlations reveal how bilingualism enhances cognitive flexibility. Cortex, 49(10), 2853-2860. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ladas, A., Carroll, D., & Vivas, A.
(2015) Attentional processes in low-socioeconomic status bilingual children: Are they modulated by the amount of bilingual experience? Child Development, 86(2), 557-578. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605-621. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., De Sa, E., & Bialystok, E.
(2011) Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 14(4), 588-595. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martin-Rhee, M., & Bialystok, E.
(2008) The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 81-93. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mezzacappa, E.
(2004) Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75(5), 1373-1386. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N.
(2012) The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions, four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8-14. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N., Emerson, M., Witzki, A., Howerter, A., & Wager, T.
(2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, T.-K., & Astington, J.
(2014) Reassessing the bilingual advantage in theory of mind and its cognitive underpinnings. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 17(2), 396-409. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nicolay, A.-C., & Poncelet, M.
(2013) Cognitive advantage in children enrolled in a second-language immersion elementary school program for three years. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 16(3), 597-607. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peirce, J.
(2007) PsychoPy-Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1-2), 8-13. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Poarch, G., & Bialystok, E.
(2015) Bilingualism as a model for multitasking. Developmental Review, 35, 113-124. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Poarch, G., & van Hell, J.
(2012) Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 535-551. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M., Rothbart, M., & Rueda, M.
(2013) Chapter 22 Developing attention and self-regulation in infancy and childhood. In J. Rakic (Ed.), Neural circuit development and function in the brain (pp. 395-411). Oxford, UK: Academic Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rueda, M.
(2013) Development of attention. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rueda, M., Fan, J., McCandliss, B., Halparin, J., Gruber, D., Lercari, L., & Posner, M.
(2004) Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 1029-1040. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Skibbe, L., Connor, C., Morrison, F., & Jewkes, A.
(2011) Schooling effects on preschoolers’ self-regulation, early literacy, and language growth. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26(1), 42-49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tao, L., Taft, M., & Gollan, T.
(2015) The bilingual switching advantage: Sometimes related to bilingual proficiency, sometimes not. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 21(7), 531-544. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tse, C.-S., & Altarriba, J.
(2014) The relationship between language proficiency and attentional control in Cantonese-English bilingual children: Evidence from Simon, Simon switching, and working memory tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 954.Google Scholar
Vega, C., & Fernandez, M.
(2011) Errors on the WCST correlate with language proficiency scores in Spanish-English bilingual children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26(2), 158-164. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wiebe, S.
(2014) Modeling the emergent executive: Implications for the structure and development of executive function. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79(2), 104-115. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wiebe, S., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J., Clark, C., Chevalier, N., & Espy, K.
(2011) The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 436-452. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wiig, E., Secord, W., & Semel, E.
(2004) CELF Preschool 2, Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preschool. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.Google Scholar
Yeniad, N., Malda, M., Mesman, J., van Ijzendoorn, M., Emmen, R., & Prevoo, M.
(2014) Cognitive flexibility children across the transition to school: A longitudinal study. Cognitive Development, 31, 35-47. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by other publications

Luk, Gigi & Judith F. Kroll
2019.  In The Cambridge Handbook of Cognition and Education,  pp. 292 ff. Crossref logo
Smith, Sara Ashley, Jessica G Briggs & Holly Pothier
2018. Exploring variation in reading comprehension among young adult Spanish–English bilinguals: The role of environmental language contact and attitudes toward reading. International Journal of Bilingualism 22:6  pp. 695 ff. Crossref logo
Smith, Sara Ashley, Jessica G Briggs, Holly Pothier & Jorge Nicholas Garcia
2019. ‘Mental Workouts for Couch Potatoes’: Executive Function Variation among Spanish–English Bilingual Young Adults. Applied Linguistics 40:3  pp. 413 ff. Crossref logo
Verhagen, Josje, Elise de Bree & Sharon Unsworth
2020. Effects of Bilingual Language Use and Language Proficiency on 24-month-olds’ Cognitive Control. Journal of Cognition and Development 21:1  pp. 46 ff. Crossref logo
Whitford, Veronica & Gigi Luk
2019.  In Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond [Studies in Bilingualism, 57],  pp. 67 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 28 december 2020. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.