Article published in:
Cognitive Control and Consequences of Multilingualism
Edited by John W. Schwieter
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 2] 2016
► pp. 427450
References

References

Abutalebi, J., & Green, D.
(2008) Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language & Cognitive Processes, 23, 557-582. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J., Estévez, A., Hernández, J., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L.,… & Carreiras, M.
(2014) Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and aging, 19(2), 290. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G.
(2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 34(4), 859. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Kroll, J., Green, Dw., MacWhinney, B., & Craik, F.
(2015) Publication bias and the validity of evidence: What’s the connection? Psychological science, 26(6), 944-946. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blumenfeld, H., & Marian, V.
(2011) Bilingualism influences inhibitory control in auditory comprehension. Cognition, 118(2), 245-257. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 547-567. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Calleja, M., & Rich, A.
(2013) Guidance of attention by information held in working memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(4), 687-699. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S., & Meltzoff, A.
(2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282-298. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Coderre, E., & Van Heuven, W.
(2014) Electrophysiological explorations of the bilingual advantage: Evidence from a Stroop task. PloS one, 9(7), e103424. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Coderre, E., Van Heuven, W., & Conklin, K.
(2012) The timing and magnitude of Stroop interference and facilitation in monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 1, 22.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135-149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dong, Y., & Xie, Z.
(2014) Contributions of second language proficiency and interpreting experience to cognitive control differences among young adult bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(5), 506-519. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Downing, P.
(2000) Interactions between visual working memory and selective attention. Psychological Science, 11(6), 467-473. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J.A., Hernández, J.A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L.J., & Carreiras, M.
(2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited. Experimental Psychology. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Friesen, D., Latman, V., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E.
(2015) Attention during visual search: The benefit of bilingualism. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19(6), 693-702. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Giezen, M., Blumenfeld, H., Shook, A., Marian, V., & Emmorey, K.
(2015) Parallel language activation and inhibitory control in bimodal bilinguals. Cognition, 141, 9-25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D., & Abutalebi, J.
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 515-530. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Heidlmayr, K., Moutier, S., Hemforth, B., Tanzmeister, R., & Isel, F.
(2014) Successive bilingualism and executive functions: The effect of second language use on inhibitory control in a behavioural Stroop colour word task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(3), 630-645. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hernández, M., Costa, A., Fuentes, L., Vivas, A., & Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2010) The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 13, 315-325. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hernández, M., Costa, A., & Humphreys, G.
(2012) Escaping capture: Bilingualism modulates distraction from working memory. Cognition, 122, 37-50. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hilchey, M., & Klein, R.
(2011) Are there bilingual advantages on non-linguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 625-658. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hodgson, T., Parris, B. Gregory, N., & Jarvis, T.
(2009) The saccadic Stroop effect: Evidence for involuntary programming of eye movements by linguistic cues. Vision Research, 49, 569-574. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, J., & Subramaniam, B.
(1995).The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 37, 787-795. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J.
(2012) The construct of language proficiency in the study of bilingualism from a cognitive perspective. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(2), 422-433. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 37-41. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Logan, G., & Cowan, W.
(1984) On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological Review, 91, 295-327. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., De Sa, E., & Bialystok, E.
(2011) Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(4), 588-595. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V. & Spivey, M.
(2003).Comparing bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 173-193. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Manamara, B., & Conway, A.
(2014) Novel evidence in support of the bilingual advantage: Influences of task demands and experience on cognitive control and working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 520-525. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mercier, J., Pivneva, I., & Titone, D.
(2014) Individual differences in inhibitory control relate to bilingual spoken word processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 89-117. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mishra, R., & Singh, S.
(2014) Activation of shape and semantic information during ambiguous homophone processing: Eye tracking evidence from Hindi. Cognitive Processing, 15(4), 451-465. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mishra, R.K.
(2015) Let's not forget about language proficiency and cultural variations while linking bilingualism to executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(01), 39-40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Monsell, S.
(2003).Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 134-140. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Olivers, C.N.
(2009) What drives memory-driven attentional capture? The effects of memory type, display type, and search type. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 1275. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Olivers, C., Meijer, F., & Theeuwes, J.
(2006) Feature-based memory-driven attentional capture: Visual working memory content affects visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(5), 1243. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K., & Greenberg, Z.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232-58. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K., & Sawi, O.
(2014) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in psychology, 5. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A.
(2012) A new form of human spatial attention: Automated symbolic orienting. Visual Cognition, 20, 244-264. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schall, J., Stuphorn, V., & Brown, J.
(2002) Monitoring and control of action by the frontal lobes. Neuron, 36, 309-322. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Singh, J., & Mishra, R.
(2015) Effect of bilingualism on anticipatory oculomotor control. International Journal of Bilingualism. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Singh, N., & Mishra, R.
(2015) Performance monitoring and response inhibition in a saccadic countermanding task in low and high proficient bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2015) Unintentional activation of translation equivalents in bilinguals leads to attention capture in a cross-modal visual task. PLoS One, 10(3), e0120131. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) Does language proficiency modulate oculomotor control? Evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 771-781. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Second language proficiency modulates conflict-monitoring in an oculomotor Stroop task: evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1-10. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Soto, D., Heinke, D., Humphreys, G., & Blanco, M.
(2005) Early, involuntary top-down guidance of attention from working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(2), 248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Soto, D., Humphreys, G., & Rotshtein, P.
(2007) Dissociating the neural mechanisms of memory-based guidance of visual selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(43), 17186-17191. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Strobach, T., Frensch, P., & Schubert, T.
(2012) Video game practice optimizes executive control skills in dual-task and task switching situations. Acta Psychologica, 140(1), 13-24. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stuphorn, V., & Schall, J.
(2006) Executive control of countermanding saccades by the supplementary eye field. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 925-931. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., & Wodniecka, Z.
(2011) The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: The role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(123), 1-19. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tse, C., & Altarriba, J.
(2012) The effects of first- and second- language proficiency on conflict resolution and goal maintenance in bilinguals: evidence from reaction time distributional analyses in a Stroop task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 663-676. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) The relationship between language proficiency and attentional control in Cantonese-English bilingual children: Evidence from Simon, Simon switching, and working memory tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 1-18.Google Scholar
Valian, V.
(2015) Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3-24. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weber, A., & Cutler, A.
(2004) Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(1), 1-25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wijnen J., & Ridderinkhof, K.
(2007) Response inhibition in motor and oculomotor conflict tasks: Different mechanisms, different dynamics? Brain and Cognition, 63, 260-270. CrossrefGoogle Scholar