References
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D
(2008) Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language & Cognitive Processes, 23, 557-582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J., Estévez, A., Hernández, J., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L.,… & Carreiras, M
(2014) Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and aging, 19(2), 290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G
(2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 34(4), 859. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Kroll, J., Green, Dw., MacWhinney, B., & Craik, F
(2015) Publication bias and the validity of evidence: What’s the connection? Psychological science, 26(6), 944-946. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blumenfeld, H., & Marian, V
(2011) Bilingualism influences inhibitory control in auditory comprehension. Cognition, 118(2), 245-257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 547-567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Calleja, M., & Rich, A
(2013) Guidance of attention by information held in working memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(4), 687-699. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S., & Meltzoff, A
(2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282-298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coderre, E., & Van Heuven, W
(2014) Electrophysiological explorations of the bilingual advantage: Evidence from a Stroop task. PloS one, 9(7), e103424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coderre, E., Van Heuven, W., & Conklin, K
(2012) The timing and magnitude of Stroop interference and facilitation in monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 1, 22.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135-149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dong, Y., & Xie, Z
(2014) Contributions of second language proficiency and interpreting experience to cognitive control differences among young adult bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(5), 506-519. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Downing, P
(2000) Interactions between visual working memory and selective attention. Psychological Science, 11(6), 467-473. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J.A., Hernández, J.A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L.J., & Carreiras, M
(2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited. Experimental Psychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friesen, D., Latman, V., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E
(2015) Attention during visual search: The benefit of bilingualism. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19(6), 693-702. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giezen, M., Blumenfeld, H., Shook, A., Marian, V., & Emmorey, K
(2015) Parallel language activation and inhibitory control in bimodal bilinguals. Cognition, 141, 9-25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D., & Abutalebi, J
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 515-530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heidlmayr, K., Moutier, S., Hemforth, B., Tanzmeister, R., & Isel, F
(2014) Successive bilingualism and executive functions: The effect of second language use on inhibitory control in a behavioural Stroop colour word task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(3), 630-645. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernández, M., Costa, A., Fuentes, L., Vivas, A., & Sebastian-Galles, N
(2010) The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 13, 315-325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernández, M., Costa, A., & Humphreys, G
(2012) Escaping capture: Bilingualism modulates distraction from working memory. Cognition, 122, 37-50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hilchey, M., & Klein, R
(2011) Are there bilingual advantages on non-linguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 625-658. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hodgson, T., Parris, B. Gregory, N., & Jarvis, T
(2009) The saccadic Stroop effect: Evidence for involuntary programming of eye movements by linguistic cues. Vision Research, 49, 569-574. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, J., & Subramaniam, B
(1995).The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 37, 787-795. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J
(2012) The construct of language proficiency in the study of bilingualism from a cognitive perspective. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(2), 422-433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J., & Bialystok, E
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 37-41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Logan, G., & Cowan, W
(1984) On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological Review, 91, 295-327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., De Sa, E., & Bialystok, E
(2011) Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(4), 588-595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marian, V. & Spivey, M
(2003).Comparing bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 173-193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Manamara, B., & Conway, A
(2014) Novel evidence in support of the bilingual advantage: Influences of task demands and experience on cognitive control and working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 520-525. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mercier, J., Pivneva, I., & Titone, D
(2014) Individual differences in inhibitory control relate to bilingual spoken word processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 89-117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mishra, R., & Singh, S
(2014) Activation of shape and semantic information during ambiguous homophone processing: Eye tracking evidence from Hindi. Cognitive Processing, 15(4), 451-465. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mishra, R.K
(2015) Let's not forget about language proficiency and cultural variations while linking bilingualism to executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(01), 39-40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Monsell, S
(2003).Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 134-140. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Olivers, C.N
(2009) What drives memory-driven attentional capture? The effects of memory type, display type, and search type. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 1275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Olivers, C., Meijer, F., & Theeuwes, J
(2006) Feature-based memory-driven attentional capture: Visual working memory content affects visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(5), 1243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K., & Greenberg, Z
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232-58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K., & Sawi, O
(2014) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in psychology, 5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A
(2012) A new form of human spatial attention: Automated symbolic orienting. Visual Cognition, 20, 244-264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schall, J., Stuphorn, V., & Brown, J
(2002) Monitoring and control of action by the frontal lobes. Neuron, 36, 309-322. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Singh, J., & Mishra, R
(2015) Effect of bilingualism on anticipatory oculomotor control. International Journal of Bilingualism. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Singh, N., & Mishra, R
(2015) Performance monitoring and response inhibition in a saccadic countermanding task in low and high proficient bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Unintentional activation of translation equivalents in bilinguals leads to attention capture in a cross-modal visual task. PLoS One, 10(3), e0120131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Does language proficiency modulate oculomotor control? Evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 771-781. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Second language proficiency modulates conflict-monitoring in an oculomotor Stroop task: evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1-10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soto, D., Heinke, D., Humphreys, G., & Blanco, M
(2005) Early, involuntary top-down guidance of attention from working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(2), 248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soto, D., Humphreys, G., & Rotshtein, P
(2007) Dissociating the neural mechanisms of memory-based guidance of visual selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(43), 17186-17191. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Strobach, T., Frensch, P., & Schubert, T
(2012) Video game practice optimizes executive control skills in dual-task and task switching situations. Acta Psychologica, 140(1), 13-24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stuphorn, V., & Schall, J
(2006) Executive control of countermanding saccades by the supplementary eye field. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 925-931. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., & Wodniecka, Z
(2011) The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: The role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(123), 1-19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tse, C., & Altarriba, J
(2012) The effects of first- and second- language proficiency on conflict resolution and goal maintenance in bilinguals: evidence from reaction time distributional analyses in a Stroop task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 663-676. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) The relationship between language proficiency and attentional control in Cantonese-English bilingual children: Evidence from Simon, Simon switching, and working memory tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 1-18.Google Scholar
Valian, V
(2015) Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3-24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weber, A., & Cutler, A
(2004) Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(1), 1-25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wijnen J., & Ridderinkhof, K
(2007) Response inhibition in motor and oculomotor conflict tasks: Different mechanisms, different dynamics? Brain and Cognition, 63, 260-270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar