Part of
Cognitive Individual Differences in Second Language Processing and Acquisition
Edited by Gisela Granena, Daniel O. Jackson and Yucel Yilmaz
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 3] 2016
► pp. 89104
References
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J.
(1974) Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation, (Vol. 8; pp. 47–90). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D.
(2003) Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chun, D. M., & Payne, J. S.
(2004) What makes students click: Working memory and look-up behavior. System, 32, 481–503. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W.
(2005) Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 12, 769–786. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P.
(1980) Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C.
(2005) At the interface: dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 305–352. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernández, C.
(2008) Reexamining the role of explicit information in processing instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30, 277–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedman, N.P., & Miyake, A.
(2005) Comparison of four scoring methods for the reading span test. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 581–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gass, S.M.
(1997) Input, interaction, and the second language learner. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Harrington, M., & Sawyer, M.
(1992) L2 working memory capacity and L2 reading skill. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 14, 25–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Irwin, J. R., & McClelland, G. H.
(2003) Negative consequences of dichotomizing continuous predictor variables. Journal of Marketing Research, 40, 366–371 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A.
(2004) Representation, processing and working memory in a second language. Transactions of the Philological Society, 102, 199–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) The influence of first language on the processing of wh-movement in English as a second language. Second Language Research, 21, 121–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J., Michael, E., Tokowicz, N., & Dufour, R.
(2002) The development of lexical fluency in a second language. Second Language Research, 18, 137–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leeser, M. J.
(2007) Learner-based factors in L2 reading comprehension and processing grammatical form: Topic familiarity and working memory. Language Learning, 57, 229–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Long, M.
(1990) The least a second language acquisition theory needs to explain. TESOL Quarterly, 24, 649–666. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacCallum, R.C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K.J., & Rucker, D.D.
(2002) On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7, 19–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, M.C., & Christiansen, M.H.
(2002) Reassessing working memory: comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109, 35–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mackey, A., Philp, J., Egi, T., Fujii, A., & Tatsumi, T.
(2002) Individual differences in working memory, noticing interactional feedback and L2 development. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Individual differences and instructed language learning (pp. 181–209). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A.
(1998) Individual differences in second language proficiency: The role of working memory. In A. F. Healy & L. E. Bourne Jr., (Eds.), Foreign language learning: Psycholinguistic studies on training and retention (pp. 339–364). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., & Shah, P.
(Eds.) (1999) Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Grady, W.
(2003) The radical middle: Nativism without universal grammar. In C. J. Doughty & M. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 43–62). Malden, MA: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Osaka, M., & Osaka, N.
(1992) Language-independent working memory as measured by Japanese and English reading span tests. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30, 287–289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Osaka, M., Osaka, N., & Groner, R.
(1993) Language-independent working memory: Evidence from German and French reading span tests. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31, 117–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pienemann, M.
Skehan, P.
(1998) A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sunderman, G., & Kroll, J.F.
(2006) First language activation during second language lexical processing: An investigation of lexical form, meaning, and grammatical class. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 387–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) When study abroad experience fails to deliver: The internal resources threshold effect. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 1–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tokowicz, N., Michael, E.B., & Kroll, J.F.
(2004) The roles of study-abroad experience and working-memory capacity in the types of errors made during translation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 255–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Turner, M., & Engle, R.W.
(1989) Is working memory task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127–154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B.
(1996) Input processing and grammar instruction: Theory and research. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
(2007) Input processing in adult second language acquisition. In B. VanPatten, & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition (pp. 115–135). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B., & Williams, J.
(2007) Introduction: The nature of theories. In B. VanPatten, & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition (pp. 1–16). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Waters, G.S., & Caplan, D.
(1996) The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to reading comprehension. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 51–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, J. P.
(2008) The effect of input-based instruction type on the acquisition of Spanish accusative clitics. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Florida State University.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 6 other publications

Edmonds, Amanda & Aarnes Gudmestad
2018. Chapter 5. Operationalizing variables. In Critical Reflections on Data in Second Language Acquisition [Language Learning & Language Teaching, 51],  pp. 125 ff. DOI logo
Edmonds, Amanda & Pascale Leclercq
2020. Introduction. Journal of French Language Studies 30:2  pp. 111 ff. DOI logo
Gudmestad, Aarnes & Amanda Edmonds
2018. Introduction. In Critical Reflections on Data in Second Language Acquisition [Language Learning & Language Teaching, 51],  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
In’nami, Yo, Yuko Hijikata & Rie Koizumi
2022. WORKING MEMORY CAPACITY AND L2 READING. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 44:2  pp. 381 ff. DOI logo
Jo, Kyuhee, Kitaek Kim & Hyunwoo Kim
2021. Children's interpretation of negation and quantifier scope in L3 English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 24:3  pp. 427 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.