Article published in:
Cognitive Individual Differences in Second Language Processing and Acquisition
Edited by Gisela Granena, Daniel O. Jackson and Yucel Yilmaz
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 3] 2016
► pp. 223248
References

References

Adams, A.-M., & Guillot, K.
(2008) Working memory and writing in bilingual students. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 156, 13–28. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Alptekin, C., & Erçetin, G.
(2010) The role of L1 and L2 working memory in literal and inferential comprehension in L2 reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 33, 206–219. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) The effects of working memory capacity and content familiarity on literal and inferential comprehension in L2 reading. TESOL Quarterly, 45, 235–266. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Alptekin, C., Erçetin, G., & Özemir, O.
(2014) Effects of variations in reading span task design on the relationship between working memory capacity and second language reading. The Modern Language Journal, 98, 536–552. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R.
(1993) Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D.
(2003) Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189–208. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2007) Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bowles, M.
(2011) Measuring implicit and explicit linguistic knowledge: What can heritage language learners contribute? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 247–271. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A.
(1989) The role of working memory in language comprehension. In D. K. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 31–68). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W.
(2005) Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 769–786. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N.
(2011) Working memory and attention in language use. In J. Guendouzi, F. Loncke, & M. J. Williams (Eds.), The handbook of psycholinguistic and cognitive processes: Perspectives in communication disorders (pp. 75–97). New York, NY: Psychology Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P.A.
(1980) Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dehn, M. J.
(2008) Working memory and academic learning: Assessment and intervention. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R.
(2007) Skill acquisition theory. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition (pp. 97–113). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2009) Cognitive-psychological processes in second language learning. In M. H. Long & C. J. Doughty (Eds.), The handbook of language teaching (pp. 119–138). Malden, MA: Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R., & Juffs, A.
(2005) Cognitive considerations in L2 learning. In E. Hinkel (Ed.), Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning (pp. 437–454). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z.
(2009) The psychology of second language acquisition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C.
(2008) Implicit and explicit knowledge about language. In J. Cenoz & N. Hornberger (Eds.), Encyclopedia of language and education (pp. 119–131). New York, NY: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ellis R.
(2009b) Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language. In R. Ellis, S. Loewen, C. Elder, R. Erlam, J. Philp, & H. Reinder (Eds.), Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language learning, testing and teaching (pp. 31–63). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Ellis, R.
(1993) Second language acquisition and the structural syllabus. TESOL Quarterly, 27, 91–113. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 141–172. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R., Loewen, S., Elder, C., Erlam, R., Philp, J., & Reinders, H.
(Eds.) (2009) Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language learning, testing and teaching. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Engle, R. W., Cantor, J., & Carullo, J. J.
(1992) Individual differences in working memory and comprehension: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 972–992.Google Scholar
Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., & Tuholski, S. W.
(1999) Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 102–134). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Erçetin, G., & Alptekin, C.
(2013) The explicit/implicit knowledge distinction and working memory: Implications for L2 reading comprehension. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 727–753. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Erlam, R.
(2006) Elicited imitation as a measure of L2 implicit knowledge: An empirical validation study. Applied Linguistics, 27, 464–491. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Geva, E., & Ryan, E. B.
(1993) Linguistic and cognitive correlates of academic skills in first and second languages. Language Learning, 43, 5–42. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goo, J.
(2012) Corrective feedback and working memory capacity in interaction-driven L2 learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34, 445–474. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grabe, W.
(2009) Reading in a second language: Moving from theory to practice. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Granena, G.
(2013) Reexamining the robustness of aptitude in second language acquisition. In G. Granena & M. Long (Eds.), Sensitive periods, language aptitude, and ultimate attainment (pp. 179–204). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Han, Y.
(2000) Grammaticality judgment tests: How reliable and valid are they? Applied Language Learning, 11, 177–204.Google Scholar
Han, Y., & Ellis, R.
(1998) Implicit knowledge, explicit knowledge, and general language proficiency. Language Teaching Research, 2, 1–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Han, Z. H., & Finneran, R.
(2014) Re-engaging the interface debate: Strong, weak, none, or all? Journal of Applied Linguistics, 24, 370–389. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, M., & Sawyer, M.
(1992) L2 working memory capacity and L2 reading skill. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 14, 25–38. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hassin, R. R., Bargh, J. A., Engell, A. D., & McCulloch, K. C.
(2009) Implicit working memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 665–678. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J. H.
(2001) Intentional and incidental second language vocabulary learning: A reappraisal of elaboration, rehearsal and automaticity. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 258–286). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N.
(2007) Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning. Language Learning, 57, 1–33. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Conducting reaction time research for second language studies. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Juffs, A., & DeKeyser, R.
(2003) Psycholinguistics. In L. Nadel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of cognitive science (pp. 1022–1028). London, UK: Nature Publishing.Google Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A.
(1992) A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 98, 122–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W.
(2000) Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: Limits on long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 336–358.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W.
(1998) Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Krashen, S.
(1985) The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. London, UK: Longman.Google Scholar
Leech, N. L., Barrett, K. C., & Morgan, G. A.
(2011) IBM SPSS for intermediate statistics: Use and interpretation. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P.
(1998) Individual differences in second language proficiency: Working memory as language aptitude. In A. F. Healy & L. E. Bourne (Eds.), Foreign language learning: Psycholinguistic studies on training and retention (pp. 339–364). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Norris, J., & Ortega, L.
(2003) Defining and measuring SLA. In C. Doughty & M. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 717–761). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) Assessing learner knowledge. In S. Gass & A. Mackey (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 573–589). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Osaka, M., & Osaka, N.
(1992) Language-independent working memory as measured by Japanese and English reading span tests. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30, 287–289. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Osaka, M., Osaka, N., & Groner, R.
(1993) Language-independent working memory: Evidence from German and French reading span tests. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 31, 117–118. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, M.
(2009) Declarative and procedural determinants of second languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Payne, T. W., Kalibatseva, Z., & Jungers, M. K.
(2009) Does domain experience compensate for working memory capacity in second language reading comprehension? Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 119–123. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roehr, K., & Gánem-Gutiérrez, G. A.
(2009) The status of metalinguistic knowledge in instructed adult L2 learning. Language Awareness, 18, 165–181. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sagarra, N.
(2013) Working memory in second language acquisition. In C. A. Chapelle (Ed.), The encyclopedia of applied linguistics. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Shah, P., & Miyake, A.
(1999) Models of working memory: An introduction. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 1–27). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sawyer, M., & Ranta, L.
(2001) Aptitude, individual differences, and instructional design. In P. Robinson (Ed.) Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 319–353). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, N.
(2003) Automaticity and second language learning. In C. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 382–408). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Serafini, E.
(2013) Cognitive and psychological factors in the long-term development of implicit and explicit second language knowledge in adult learners of Spanish at increasing proficiency. Unpublished dissertation. Georgetown University. Google Scholar
Service, E., Simola, M., Metsaenheimo, O., & Maury, S.
(2002) Bilingual working memory span is affected by language skill. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 383–407. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shiu, L. J., & Spada, N.
(2012) An empirical validation of elicited imitation as a measure of L2 implicit knowledge. Paper presented at the International Association of Applied Linguistics (AILA) Congress, Beijing, CN, August.
Spada, N.
(2015) SLA research and L2 pedagogy: Misapplications and questions of relevance. Language Teaching, 48, 69–81. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, Y., & DeKeyser, R.
(2015) Comparing elicited imitation and word monitoring as measures of implicit knowledge. Language Learning, 65, 860–895. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sweller, J.
(1994) Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty and instructional design. Learning and Instruction, 4, 295–312. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S.
(2007) Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.Google Scholar
Towse, J. N., & Hitch, G. J.
(1995) Is there a relationship between task demand and storage space in tests of working memory capacity? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 108–124. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Towse, J. N., Hitch, G. J., & Hutton, U.
(2002) On the nature of the relationship between processing activity and item retention in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 156–184. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W.
(1989) Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127–154. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T.
(2004) Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231–270. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) A cognitive neuroscience perspective on second language acquisition: The declarative/procedural model. In C. Sanz (Ed.), Mind and context in adult second language acquisition: Methods, theory, and practice (pp. 141–178). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Van den Noort, M., Bosch, P., & Hugdahl, K.
(2006) Foreign language proficiency and working memory capacity. European Psychologist, 11, 289–296. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yilmaz, Y.
(2013) Relative effects of explicit and implicit feedback: The role of working memory capacity and language analytic ability. Applied Linguistics, 34, 344–368. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Walter, C.
(2004) Transfer of reading comprehension skills to L2 is linked to mental representations of text and to L2 working memory. Applied Linguistics, 25, 315–339. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D.
(1996) The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to reading comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 51–79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar