Chapter published in:
L3 Syntactic Transfer: Models, new developments and implications
Edited by Tanja Angelovska and Angela Hahn
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 5] 2017
► pp. 85101
References

References

Baddeley, A.
(2000) The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(11), 417–423. Crossref.Google Scholar
(2003) Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(3), 189–208. Crossref.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G.
(1974) Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bardel, C.
(in progress). Function words and other words in third language production. Ms.
Bardel, C., & Falk, Y.
(2007) The role of the second language in third language acquisition: The case of Germanic syntax. Second Language Research, 23(4), 459–484. Crossref.Google Scholar
(2012) The L2 status factor and the declarative/procedural distinction. In J. Cabrelli Amaro, S. Flynn & J. Rothman (Eds.), Third language acquisition in adulthood (Studies in Bilingualism 46) (pp. 61–78). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(1994a) Representation and ways of knowing: Three issues in second language acquisition. In N. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit factors in second language learning: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 549–569). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
(1994b) Analysis and control in the development of second language proficiency. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16(2), 157–168. Crossref.Google Scholar
(2001) Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Ryan, E. B.
(1985) Toward a definition of metalinguistic skill. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 31, 229–251.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D.
(1989) Metalinguistic performance and interlinguistic competence. Berlin: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bohnacker, U.
(2005) Nonnative acquisition of verb second: On the empirical underpinnings of universal L2 claims. In M. den Dikken & C. M. Tortora (Eds.), The function of function words and functional categories (Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today 78) (pp. 41–77). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006) When Swedes begin to learn German: From V2 to V2. Second Language Research, 22(4), 443–86. Crossref.Google Scholar
Conway, A., Tuholski, S., Shisler, R., & Engle, R.
(1999) The effect of memory load on negative priming: An individual differences investigation. Memory and Cognition, 27(6), 1042–1050. Crossref.Google Scholar
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M.
(2004) Lexical access in bilingual production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(4), 491–511. Crossref.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M.
(2003) Implicit and explicit learning. In C. J. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 313 –348). Malden, MA: Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dewaele, J. M.
(2001) Activation or inhibition? The interaction of L1, L2, and L3 on the language mode continuum. In J. Cenoz, B. Hufeisen, & U. Jessner (Eds.), Cross-linguistic influence in third language acquisition: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 69–89). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z.
(2009) The psychology of second language acquisition. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Ecke, P., & Hall., C. J.
(2000) Lexikalischer Fehler in Deutsch als Drittsprache: Translexikalischer Einfluss auf 3 Ebenen der mentalen Repräsentation. Deutsch als Fremdsprache, (37)1, 30–36.Google Scholar
Ellis, N.
(2001) Memory for language. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 33–68). Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R.
(2006) Modelling learning difficulty and second language proficiency: The differential contributions of implicit and explicit knowledge. Applied Linguistics, 27(3), 431–463. Crossref.Google Scholar
Engle, R.
(2002) Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 19–23. Crossref.Google Scholar
Falk, Y., & Bardel, C.
(2010) The study of the role of the background languages in third language acquisition: The state of the art. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 48(2–3), 185–220. Crossref.Google Scholar
(2011) Object pronouns in German L3 syntax: Evidence for the L2 status factor. Second Language Research, 27(1), 59–82. Crossref.Google Scholar
Falk, Y., Lindqvist, C., & Bardel, C.
(2015) The role of L1 explicit metalinguistic knowledge in L3 oral production at the initial state. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 227–235. Crossref.Google Scholar
Fehringer, C., & Fry, C.
(2007) Hesitation phenomena in the language production of bilingual speakers. Folia Linguistica, 41(1–2), 37–72.Google Scholar
Gass, S.
(1988) Integrating research areas: A framework for second language studies. Applied Linguistics, 9(2), 198–217. Crossref.Google Scholar
Green, D.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 67–81. Crossref.Google Scholar
Hammarberg, B.
(2009) Processes in third language acquisition. Edinburgh: EUP.Google Scholar
(2010) The languages of the multilingual: Some conceptual and terminological issues. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 48(2–3), 91–104. Crossref.Google Scholar
(2016) Flerspråkighet och tredjespråksinlärning. Några grundbegrepp [Multilingualism and third language learning. Some fundamental concepts]. In C. Bardel, Y. Falk, & C. Lindqvist, Tredjespråksinlärning [Third language learning] (pp. 33–58). Lund: Studentlitteratur.Google Scholar
Kane, M., & Engle, R.
(2003) Working memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(1), 47–70. Crossref.Google Scholar
Kellerman, E.
(1983) Now you see it, now you don’t. In S. Gass & L. Selinker (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp. 112–134). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Kormos, J.
(2013) New conceptualization of language aptitude in second language attainment. In G. Granena & M. Long (Eds.), Sensitive periods, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment (Language Learning & Language Acquisition 35) (pp. 131–152). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J., Bobb, S., & Wodniecka, Z.
(2006) Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: Arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9(2), 119–135. Crossref.Google Scholar
Kroll, J., Boo, S., Misra, M., & Guo, T.
(2008) Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta psychological, 128(3), 416–430. Crossref.Google Scholar
Leung, Y.-k. I.
(2002) L2 vs. L3 initial state: Evidence from the acquisition of French DPs by Vietnamese monolinguals and Cantonese – English bilinguals. In M. Hooghiemstra (Ed.), Proceedings of the second international conference on third language acquisition and trilingualism. Interactive CD-rom: L3 conference. Ljowert & Leeuwarden: Fryske Akademy.Google Scholar
Lindqvist, C., & Falk, Y.
(2014) When Germans begin to learn Swedish: Which is the transfer source for function words, content words and syntax? Eurosla Yearbook, 14, 225–239. Crossref.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N., & Emerson, M.
(2000) The unity and diversity of functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. Crossref.Google Scholar
Gilabert, R., & Muñoz, C.
(2010) Differences in attainment and performance in a foreign language: The role of working memory capacity. International Journal of English Studies, 10(1), 19–42. Crossref.Google Scholar
Odlin, T.
(1989) Language Transfer. Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, M.
(2004) A neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism (Studies in Bilingualism 18). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) Language and communication disorders in multilinguals. In B. Stemmer & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Handbook of the neuroscience of language (pp. 341–349). Amsterdam: Elsevier. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Payne, J., & Whitney, P.
(2002) Developing L2 oral proficiency through synchronous CMC: Output, working memory, and interlanguage development. CALICO Journal, 20(1), 7–32.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M.
(2015) When L1 becomes an L3: Do heritage speakers make better L3 learners? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 163–178. Crossref.Google Scholar
Roberts, R., Hager, L., & Heron, C.
(1994) Prefrontal cognitive processes: working memory and inhibition in the antisaccade task. Journal of Experimenal Psychology: General, 123(4), 374–393. Crossref.Google Scholar
Robinson, P.
(2003) Attention and memory in SLA. In C. Doughty, & M. Long (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 631–678). Malden, MA: Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Individual differences, aptitude complexes, SLA processes and the development of language learning aptitude tests. In M. Pawlak (Ed.), New Perspectives on individual differences in language learning and teaching (pp. 89–109). Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
(2016) Attention and awareness. In J. Cenoz & D. Gorter (Eds.), Encyclopedia of language and education (3rd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Roehr, K., & Gánem-Gutiérrez, G. A.
(2009) The status of metalinguistic knowledge in instructed adult L2 learning. Language Awareness, 18(2), 165–181. Crossref.Google Scholar
Sáfár, A., & Kormos, J.
(2008) Revisiting problems with foreign language aptitude. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 46(2), 113–16. Crossref.Google Scholar
Sagarra, N., & Herschensohn, J.
(2010) The role of proficiency and working memory in gender and number agreement processing in L1 and L2 Spanish. Lingua, 120(8), 2022–2039. Crossref.Google Scholar
Sánchez, L.
(2011) Crosslinguistic influence in third language acquisition: English after German in Spanish Catalan learners in an instructed setting. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Barcelona.Google Scholar
(2012) ‘Luisa and Pedrito’s dog will the breakfast eat’: Interlanguage transfer and the role of the second language factor. In G. De Angelis & J. M. Dewaele (Eds.), New trends in crosslinguistic influence and multilingualism research (pp. 86–104). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
(2014) An inquiry into the role of L3 proficiency on crosslinguistic influence in third language acquisition. Odisea, Revista de Estudios Ingleses, 15, 169–188.Google Scholar
(2015) Can a background language alter the path of acquisition of verb placement in a multilingual context? Evidence from a longitudinal study. In G. De Angelis, U. Jessner, & M. Krésic (Eds.), Crosslinguistic influence and crosslinguistic interaction in language learning (pp. 71–94). London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
(under review). Crosslinguistic influence and working memory in L3 acquisition: A Correlational analysis.
Sánchez, L., & Bardel, C.
2016. Cognitive factors, perceptions and transfer in third language learning. Revue française de linguistique appliquée 2(21), 123–138.
Serafini, E., & Sanz, C.
(2015) Evidence for the decreasing impact of cognitive ability on second language development as proficiency increases. Studies in Second Language Acquisition. Available online at Crossref.
Sanz, C., Lin, H. J., Lado, B., Stafford, C., & Bowden, H.
(2014) One size fits all? Learning conditions and working memory capacity in ab initio language development. Applied Linguistics Crossref.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R.
(2001) Attention. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 3–32). Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shanon, B.
(1991) Faulty language selection in polyglots. Language and Cognitive Processes, 6(4), 339–350. Crossref.Google Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M.
(2010) Metalinguistic processing and acquisition within the MOGUL framework. In M. Everaert, T. Lentz, H. Mulder, Ø. Nilsen, & A. Zondervan (Eds.), The linguistics enterprise: From knowledge of language to knowledge in linguistics (Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today 150) (pp. 327–344). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M., & Truscott, J.
(2005) Stages or continua in second language acquisition: A MOGUL solution. Applied Linguistics, 26(2), 219–240. Crossref.Google Scholar
Skehan, P.
(2001) Tasks and language performance assessment. In M. Bygate, P. Skehan, & M. Swai (Eds.), Researching pedagogic tasks: Second language learning, teaching, and testing (pp. 167–185). London: Longman.Google Scholar
Stewart, C.
(2002) Memory. In E. Perry, H. Ashton, & A. Young. (Eds.), Neurochemistry of consciousness. Neurotransmitters in mind (Advances in Consciousness Research 36) (pp. 65–81). Amsterdam. John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sumner, M., & Samuel, A.
(2007) Lexical inhibition and sublexical facilitation are surprisingly long lasting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 33(4), 769–790. Crossref.Google Scholar
Tagarelli, K., Borges Mota, M., & Rebuschat, P.
(2014) Working memory, learning conditions, and the acquisition of L2 snytax. In W. Zhisheng, M. Borges Mota, & A. McNeill (Eds.), Working memory in second language acquisition and processing: Theory, research and commentary (pp. 224–247). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Tellier, A., & Roehr-Brackin, K.
(2013) Metalinguistic awareness in children with differing language learning experience. In Eurosla Yearbook, 13, 81–108. Crossref.Google Scholar
Thomas, J.
(1988) The role played by metalinguistic awareness in second and third language learning. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 9(3), 235–246. Crossref.Google Scholar
Trude, A., & Tokowicz, N.
(2011) Negative transfer from Spanish and English to Portuguese pronunciation: The roles of inhibition and working memory. Language Learning, 61(1), 259–280. Crossref.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T.
(2005) A cognitive neuroscience perspective on second language acquisition: The declarative/procedural model. In C. Sanz (Ed.), Mind and context in adult second language acquisition, (pp. 141–178). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Williams, S., & Hammarberg, B.
(1998) Language switches in L3 production: Implications for a polyglot model. Applied Linguistics, 19(3), 295–333. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Young, B., & Pigott, S.
(1999) Neurobiological basis of consciousness. Archives of Neurology, 56(2), 153–157. Crossref.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by other publications

Angelovska, Tanja & Angela Hahn
2017.  In L3 Syntactic Transfer [Bilingual Processing and Acquisition, 5],  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Foryś-Nogala, Małgorzata, Olga Broniś, Marcin Opacki & Agnieszka Otwinowska
2020. Cross-linguistic influences, language proficiency and metalinguistic knowledge in L3 Italian subject placement. International Journal of Multilingualism  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
González Alonso, Jorge & Jason Rothman
2017.  In L3 Syntactic Transfer [Bilingual Processing and Acquisition, 5],  pp. 277 ff. Crossref logo
Jo, Kyuhee, Kitaek Kim & Hyunwoo Kim
2020. Children's interpretation of negation and quantifier scope in L3 English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Lago, Sol, Michela Mosca & Anna Stutter Garcia
2020. The Role of Crosslinguistic Influence in Multilingual Processing: Lexicon Versus Syntax. Language Learning Crossref logo
Puig-Mayenco, Eloi, Jorge González Alonso & Jason Rothman
2020. A systematic review of transfer studies in third language acquisition. Second Language Research 36:1  pp. 31 ff. Crossref logo
Puig-Mayenco, Eloi, Jason Rothman & Susagna Tubau
2020. Language dominance in the previously acquired languages modulates rate of third language (L3) development over time: a longitudinal study. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Rothman, Jason, Jorge González Alonso & Eloi Puig-Mayenco
2019.  In Third Language Acquisition and Linguistic Transfer, Crossref logo
Saturno, Jacopo
2020. Production of inflectional morphology in intercomprehension-based language teaching: the case of Slavic languages. International Journal of Multilingualism  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Westergaard, Marit
2019. Microvariation in multilingual situations: The importance of property-by-property acquisition. Second Language Research  pp. 026765831988411 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 29 october 2020. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.