References
Al-Kasey, T., & Pérez-Leroux, A. T.
(1998) Second language acquisition of Spanish null subjects. In S. Flynn, G. Martohardjono, & W. O’Neil (Eds.), The generative study of second language acquisition (pp. 161–185). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bardel, C., & Falk, Y.
(2007) The role of the second language in third language acquisition: The case of Germanic syntax. Second Language Research, 23(4), 459–484. DOI logo.Google Scholar
(2012) Behind the L2 status factor: A neurolinguistic framework for L3 research. In J. Cabrelli Amaro, S. Flynn, & J. Rothman (Eds.), Third language acquisition in adulthood (Studies in Bilingualism 46) (pp. 61–78). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bardel, C., & Sánchez, L.
(2017) The L2 status factor hypothesis revisited: The role of metalinguistic knowledge, working memory, attention and noticing in third language learning. In T. Angelovska & A. Hahn (Eds.), L3 Syntactic Transfer: Models, new developments and implications (pp. 85–101). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berkes, É., & Flynn, S.
(2012) Further evidence in support of the Cumulative-Enhancement Model. In J. Cabrelli Amaro, S. Flynn, & J. Rothman (Eds.), Third language acquisition in adulthood (Studies in Bilingualism 46) (pp. 143–164). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borer, H., & Wexler, K.
(1987) The maturation of syntax. In T. Roeper & E. Williams (Eds.), Parameter setting (pp. 123–172). Dordrecht: Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J.
(2013) Why theory and research are important for the practice of teaching. In J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), Innovative research and practices in second language acquisition and bilingualism (Language Learning & Language Teaching 38) (pp. 85–106). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J.
(2015) Does the source of transfer affect the rate of L3 morphosyntactic development? Paper presented at the 40th Boston University Conference in Language Development. Boston, MA.
Camacho, J.
(2014) Null Subjects. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
De Angelis, G.
(2007) Third or additional language acquisition. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
De Angelis, G., & Dewaele, J.-M.
(Eds.) (2011) New trends in crosslinguistic influence and multilingualism research. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
de Bot, K., & Jaensch, C.
(2015) What is special about L3 processing? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 130–144. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Eisenstein, M.
(1980) Childhood bilingualism and adult language learning aptitude. Applied Psychology, 29(1–2), 159–172. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Epstein, S., Flynn, S., & Martohardjono, G.
(1996) Second language acquisition: Theoretical and experimental issues in contemporary research. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(4), 677–714. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Falk, Y., & Bardel, C.
(2010) The study of the role of the background languages in third language acquisition. The state of the art. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL), 48(2–3), 185–220. DOI logo.Google Scholar
(2011) Object pronouns in German L3 syntax: Evidence for the L2 status factor. Second Language Research, 27(1), 59–82. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Falk, Y., Lindqvist, C., & Bardel, C.
(2015) The role of L1 explicit metalinguistic knowledge in L3 oral production at the initial state. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 227–235. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Felix, S., & Hahn, A.
(1985) Natural processes in classroom second-language learning. Applied Linguistics, 6(3), 223–238. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Flynn, S., Foley, C., & Vinnitskaya, I.
(2004) The cumulative-enhancement model for language acquisition: Comparing adults’ and children’s patterns of development in first, second and third language acquisition. International Journal of Multilingualism, 1(1), 3–16. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Foote, R.
(2009) Transfer and L3 acquisition: The role of typology. In Y. I. Leung (Ed.), Third language acquisition and Universal Grammar (pp. 89–114). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
García Mayo, M. del P., & Rothman, J.
(2012) L3 morphosyntax in the generative tradition: From the initial state and beyond. In J. Cabrelli Amaro, S. Flynn, & J. Rothman (Eds.), Third anguage acquisition in adulthood (Studies in Bilingualism 46) (pp. 9–32). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giancaspro, D., Halloran, B., & Iverson, M.
(2015) Transfer at the initial stages of L3 Brazilian Portuguese: A look at three groups of English/Spanish bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 191–207. DOI logo.Google Scholar
González Alonso, J., & Rothman, J.
(2016) Coming of age in L3 initial stages transfer models: Deriving developmental predictions and looking towards the future. International Journal of Bilingualism. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haegeman, L., & Ihsane, T.
(2001) Adult Null Subjects in the non-pro-drop Languages: Two diary dialects. Language Acquisition, 9(4), 329–346. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Hermas, A.
(2010) Language acquisition as computational resetting: Verb movement in L3 initial state. International Journal of Multilingualism, 7(4), 343–362. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Hilles, S.
(1986) Interlanguage and the pro-drop parameter. Second Language Research, 2, 33–52. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Iverson, M.
(2010) Informing the age of acquisition debate: L3 as a litmus test. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL), 48(2–3), 221–243. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Judy, T., & Rothman, J.
(2010) From a superset to a subset grammar and the Semantic Compensation Hypothesis: Subject pronoun and anaphora resolution evidence in L2 English. In K. Franich, K. M. Iserman, & L. L. Keil (Eds.), BUCLD 34: 34th Boston University Annual Conference on Language Development (pp. 197–208). Sommerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Klein, W., & Perdue, C.
(1997) The basic variety (or: Couldn’t natural languages be much simpler?). Second Language Research, 13(4), 301–347. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D.
(1982) Principles and practice in second language acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Leung, Y. I.
(2001) The initial state of L3A: Full transfer and failed features? In C. Higgins & H. Nguyen (Eds.), The past, present and future of second language research: Selected Proceedings of the 2000 Second Language Research Forum (pp. 55–75). Sommerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. M.
(1983) Markedness, contrastive analysis and the acquisition of Spanish syntax by English speakers. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Long, D., & Rothman, J.
(2013) Generative approaches and the competing systems hypothesis: Formal acquisition to pedagogical application. In J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), Innovative research and practices in second language acquisition and bilingualism (Language Learning & Language Teaching 38) (pp. 63–84). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lozano, C.
(2002) The interpretation of overt and null pronouns in non-native Spanish. In H. Marsden, S. Pourcel, & M. Whong-Bharr (Eds.), Durham working papers in linguistics 8 (pp. 53–66). Sommerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Dias, R., & Santos, H.
(2011) Clitics and object expression in the L3 acquisition of Brazilian Portuguese: Structural similarity matters for transfer. Second Language Research, 27(1), 21–58. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Na Ranong, S., & Leung, Y. I.
(2009) Null objects in L1 Thai-L2 English-L3 Chinese: An empiricist take on a theoretical problem. In Y. I. Leung (Ed.), Third language acquisition and Universal Grammar (pp. 162–91). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T., & Glass, W. R.
(1999) Null anaphora in Spanish second language acquisition: Probabilistic versus generative approaches. Second Language Research, 15(2), 220–249. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Phinney, M.
(1987) The pro-drop parameter in second language acquisition. In T. Roeper & E. Williams (Eds.), Parameter setting (pp. 221–238). Dordrecht: Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Platzack, C.
(1996) The initial hypothesis of syntax: A minimalist perspective on language acquisition and attrition. In H. Clahsen (Ed.), Generative perspectives on language acquisition (Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 14) (pp. 369–414). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Pragmatic deficits with syntactic consequences? L2 pronominal subjects and the syntax-pragmatics interface. Journal of Pragmatics, 41(5), 951–973. DOI logo.Google Scholar
(2010) On the typological economy of syntactic transfer: Word order and relative clause high/low attachment preference in L3 Brazilian Portuguese. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL), 48(2–3), 245–273. DOI logo.Google Scholar
(2011) L3 syntactic transfer selectivity and typological determinacy: The Typological Primacy Model. Second Language Research, 27(1), 107–127. DOI logo.Google Scholar
(2013) Cognitive economy, non-redundancy and typological primacy in L3 acquisition: Evidence from initial stages of L3 Romance. In S. Baauw, F. Drijkoningen, & M. Pinto (Eds.), Romance languages and linguistic theory 2011 (Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory 5) (pp. 217–247). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Linguistic and cognitive motivations for the Typological Primacy Model (TPM) of third language (L3) transfer: Timing of acquisition and proficiency considered. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 179–190. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., Alemán Bañón, J., & González Alonso, J.
(2015) Neurolinguistic measures of typological effects in multilingual transfer: Introducing an ERP methodology. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(1087), 1–14. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., & Cabrelli Amaro, J.
(2010) What variables condition syntactic transfer? A look at the L3 initial state. Second Language Research, 26(2), 189–218. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., Cabrelli Amaro, J., & de Bot, K.
(2013) Third language acquisition. In J. Herschensohn & M. Young-Scholten (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 372–393). Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., & Halloran, B.
(2013) Formal linguistic approaches to L3/Ln acquisition: A focus on morphosyntactic transfer in adult multilingualism. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 33, 51–67. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., & Iverson, M.
(2007) On parameter clustering and resetting the null-subject parameter in L2 Spanish: Implications and observations. Hispania, 90(2), 328–341.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., Iverson, M., & Judy, T.
(2011) Some notes on the generative study of L3 acquisition. Second Language Research, 27(1), 5–19. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. D.
(1996) Now for some facts, with a focus on development and an explicit role for the L1. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(4), 739–740. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. D., & Sprouse, R. A.
(1996) L2 cognitive states and the Full Transfer/Full Access model. Second Language Research, 12(1), 40–72. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Singleton, D.
(1987) Mother and other tongue influence on learner French. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 9(3), 327–346. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R.
(2016b) The scalpel model of third language acquisition. International Journal of Bilingualism. DOI logo.Google Scholar
(2015) Is there a firewall between declarative knowledge and procedural knowledge of the functional morphology? A response to Paradis. Foreign Language Teaching and Research, 47, 1–5.Google Scholar
(2016a) Second language acquisition. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T.
(2005) A cognitive neuroscience perspective on second language acquisition: The declarative/procedural model. In C. Sanz (Ed.), Mind and context in adult second language acquisition: Methods, theory and practice (pp. 141–178). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B.
(2005) Processing Instruction. In C. Sanz (Ed.), Mind and context in adult second language acquisition: Methods, theory, and practice (pp. 267–281). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
(2010) Some verbs are more perfect than others: Why learners have difficulty with ser and estar and what it means for instruction. Hispania, 93(1), 29–38.Google Scholar
(2015) Foundations of processing instruction. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 53(2), 91–109. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Westergaard, M., Mitrofanova, N., Mykhaylyk, R., & Rodina, Y.
(2016) Crosslinguistic influence in the acquisition of a third language: The Linguistic Proximity Model. International Journal of Bilingualism. Epub ahead of print, May 19 2016 doi: DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whong, M., Gil, K.-H., & Marsden, H.
(Eds.) (2013) Universal Grammar and the second language classroom. Dordrecht: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Beyond paradigm: The “what” and the “how” of classroom research. Second Language Research, 30(4), 551–568. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Williams, S., & Hammarberg, B.
(1998) Language switches in L3 production: Implications for a polyglot speaking model. Applied Linguistics, 19(3), 295–333. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 7 other publications

Albirini, Abdulkafi, Eman Saadah & Mohammad T. Alhawary
2020. L1 and L2 transfer to L3 in L3 and L2 learners of Standard Arabic. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 10:6  pp. 745 ff. DOI logo
Angelovska, Tanja
2019. InstructedL3Acquisition ofEnglish. In The TESOL Encyclopedia of English Language Teaching,  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Angelovska, Tanja
2020. From L3 Acquisition to Teaching English. In The TESOL Encyclopedia of English Language Teaching,  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Angelovska, Tanja, Dietmar Roehm & Sabrina Weinmüller
2023. Uncovering transfer effects of dominance and proficiency in L3 English acquisition using the visual moving window paradigm and grammaticality judgments. Applied Linguistics Review 14:1  pp. 115 ff. DOI logo
Hahn, Angela & Tanja Angelovska
2017. Chapter 13. Input-Practice-Output. In L3 Syntactic Transfer [Bilingual Processing and Acquisition, 5],  pp. 299 ff. DOI logo
Rothman, Jason, Jorge González Alonso & Eloi Puig-Mayenco
2019. Third Language Acquisition and Linguistic Transfer, DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.