References
Alvarez, Al
2005A Writer’s Voice. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Bates, Elisabeth
1979The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bateson, Marie C
1979 “The epigenesis of conversational interaction: A personal account of research development.” In Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication, Margaret Bullowa (ed.), 63–77. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bernstein, Nikolaĭ A
1967Coordination and Regulation of Movements. New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Birdwhistell, Ray
1970Kinesics and Context. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Bjørkvold, Jon-Roar
1992The Muse Within: Creativity and Communication, Song and Play from Childhood through Maturity. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Bogen, Joseph E. and Bogen, Glenda M
1969 “The corpus callosum and creativity.” Bulletin of the Los Angeles Neurological Society 34: 191–220.Google Scholar
Brandt, Per A
2009 “Music and how we became human – A view from cognitive semiotics: Exploring imaginative hypotheses.” In Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship, Stephen Malloch and Colwyn Trevarthen (eds), 31–44. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bråten, Stein
1988 “Between dialogical mind and monological reason: Postulating the virtual other.” In Between Rationality and Cognition, Miriam Campanella (ed.), 205–235. Turin: Albert Meynier.Google Scholar
1992 “The virtual other in infants’ minds and social feelings.” In The Dialogical Alternative (Festschrift for Ragnar Rommetveit), Astri H. Wold (ed.), 77–97. Oslo/Oxford: Scandinavian University Press/Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
2007On Being Moved: From Mirror Neurons to Empathy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruner, Jerome S
1983Child’s Talk. Learning to Use Language. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
1986Actual Minds, Possible Worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
1990Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
2003Making Stories: Law, Literature, Life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam
1965Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cobley, Paul
2001Narrative. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Condon, Wiliam S. and Osgton, Wiliam
1971 “Speech and body motion synchrony of the speaker-hearer.” In The Perception of Language, David Horton and James Jenkins (eds), 150–184. Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill.Google Scholar
Cowley, Stephen J
2004 “Simulating others: The basis of human cognition?Language Sciences 26 (3): 273–299. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cowley, Stephen J., Moodley, Sheshni and Fiori-Cowley, Agnes
2004 “Grounding signs of culture: Primary intersubjectivity in social semiosis.” Mind, Culture and Activity 11 (2): 109–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cross, Ian and Morley, Iain
2009 “The evolution of music: Theories, definitions and the nature of the evidence.” In Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship, Stephen Malloch and Colwyn Trevarthen (eds), 61–81. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, Antonio R
1999The Feeling of What Happens: Body, Emotion and the Making of Consciousness. London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
2003Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain. San Diego: Harcourt.Google Scholar
2005 “The neurobiological grounding of human values.” In Neurobiology of Human Values, Jean-Pierre Changeux, Antonio R. Damasio, Wolf Singer and Yves Christen (eds), 47–56. Heidelberg: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Damasio, Antonio R. and Damasio, Hanna
1992 “Brain and language.” Scientific American 267: 63–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Darwin, Charles
1872The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals. London: Methuen. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1877 “A biographical sketch of an infant.” Mind 2 (7): 285–294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeCasper, Anthony J. and Prescott, Phyllis
2009 “Lateralized processes constrain auditory reinforcement in human newborns.” Hearing Research 255: 135–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Decety, Jean and Chaminade, Thierry
2003 “Neural correlates of feeling sympathy.” Neuropsychologia 41: 127–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Decety, Jean and Ickes, William
2009The Social Neuroscience of Empathy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Delafield-Butt, Jonathan and Trevarthen, Colwyn
2013 “Theories of the development of human communication.” In Theories and Models of Communication: Handbook of Communication Science, vol. 1, Paul Cobley and Peter J. Schultz (eds), 199–221. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dissanayake, Ellen
2000Art and Intimacy: How the Arts Began. Seattle/London: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
2009 “Root, leaf, blossom, or bole: Concerning the origin and adaptive function of music.” In Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship, Stephen Malloch and Colwyn Trevarthen (eds), 17–30. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
2011 “Prelinguistic and preliterate substrates of poetic narrative.” Poetics Today 32 (1): 55–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Domínguez, Duque, Turner, Robert, Lewis, E. Douglas and Egan, Gary
2010 “Neuroanthropology: A humanistic science for the study of the culture-brain nexus.” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 138–147. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Donald, Merlin
2001A Mind So Rare: The Evolution of Human Consciousness. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Donaldson, Margaret
1978Children’s Minds. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
1992Human Minds: An Exploration. London: Allen Lane/Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Dunn, Judy
1988The Beginnings of Social Understanding. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eckerdal, Patricia and Merker, Björn
2009 “’Music’ and the ‘action song’ in infant development: An interpretation.” In Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship, Stephen Malloch and Colwyn Trevarthen (eds), 241–262. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fónagy, Ivan
2001Languages within Language: An Evolutive Approach. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foolen, Ad, Lüdtke, Ulrike M., Racine Timothy P. and Zlatev, Jordan
Frank, Bodo and Trevarthen, Colwyn
2012 “Intuitive meaning: Supporting impulses for interpersonal life in the sociosphere of human knowledge, practice and language.” In Moving Ourselves, Moving Others: Motion and Emotion in Intersubjectivity, Consciousness and Language, Ad Foolen, Ulrike M. Lüdtke, Timothy P. Racine and Jordan Zlatev (eds), 261–303. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallese, Vittorio
2003 “The roots of empathy: The shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity.” Psychopathology 36: 171–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2008 “Mirror neurons and the social nature of language: The neural exploitation hypothesis.” Social Neuroscience 3: 317–333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2013 “Introduction.” In The Birth of Intersubjectivity, Massimo Ammaniti and Vittorio Gallese (eds). New York: W.W. Norton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gratier, Maya and Apter-Danon, Gisèle
2009 “The improvised musicality of belonging: Repetition and variation in mother-infant vocal interaction.” In Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship, Stephen Malloch and Colwyn Trevarthen (eds), 301–327. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gratier, Maya and Trevarthen, Colwyn
2008 “Musical narrative and motives for culture in mother-infant vocal interaction.” The Journal of Consciousness Studies 15 (10–11): 122–158.Google Scholar
Halliday, Michael A.K
1975Learning How to Mean. London: Edward Arnold. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1978Language as Social Semiotics. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
1979 “One child’s protolanguage.” In Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication, Margaret Bullowa (ed.), 171–190. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
1983 “On the transition from child tongue to mother tongue.” Australian Journal of Linguistics 3: 201–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1993 “Towards a language-based theory of learning.” Linguistics and Education 5: 93–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(ed.) 2006The Language of Science [The Collected Works of Michael A.K. Halliday, Jonathan Webster 5]. London/New York: Continuum International.Google Scholar
Halliday, Michael A.K. and Matthiessen, Christian M.I.M
2004An Introduction to Functional Grammar, (3rd ed.). London:Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Han, Shihui and Northoff, Georg
2008 “Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: A transcultural neuroimaging approach.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9: 646–654. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hobson, Robert P
2005 “What puts the jointness in joint attention?” In Joint Attention: Communication and Other Minds. Issues in Philosophy and Psychology, Naomi Eilan, Christoph Hoerl, Teresa McCormack and Johannes Roessler (eds), 185–204. Oxford: Clarendon Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hubley, Penelope and Trevarthen, Colwyn
1979 “Sharing a task in infancy.” In Social Interaction During Infancy: New Directions for Child Development 4, Ina Uzgiris (ed.), 57–80. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Hutcheson, Francis
1755A System of Moral Philosophy. Glasgow: printed and sold by Robert and Andrew Foulis. London: sold by Andrew Millar and by Thomas Longman.Google Scholar
Keltner, Dacher
2003 “Expression and the course of life: Studies of emotion, personality, and psychopathology from a social functional perspective.” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1000: 222–243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, Adam
2011 “Vocalisation, speech, gesture and the language origins debate.” Gesture 11 (3): 349–370. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kugiumutzakis, Giannis
1998 “Neonatal imitation in the intersubjective companion space.” In Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny, Stein Bråten (ed.), 63–88. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, Georg and Johnson, Mark
1999Philosophy in the Flesh. The Embodied Mind and Its Challenges to Western Thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Langer, Susanne K
1953Feeling and Form: A Theory of Art Developed from Philosophy in a New Key. London: Routledge and Kegan.Google Scholar
Lashley, Karl S
1951 “The problem of serial order in behavior.” In Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior, Lloyd A. Jeffress (ed.), 112–136. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Lee, David N
2005 “Tau in action in development.” In Action as an Organizer of Learning and Development, John J. Rieser, Jeffrey J. Lockman and Charles A. Nelson (eds), 3–49. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, Eric
1967Biological Foundations of Language. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Locke, John L
1993The Child’s Path to Spoken Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Macmurray, John
1959The Self as Agent (Volume I of the Form of the Personal). London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
1961Persons in Relation (Volume II of the Form of the Personal). London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Malloch, Stephen
1999 “Mother and infants and communicative musicality.” In Rhythms, Musical Narrative, and the Origins of Human Communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 1999–2000, Irène Deliège (ed.), 29–57. Liège, Belgium: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music.Google Scholar
Malloch, Stephen and Trevarthen, Colwyn
2009Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Maturana, Humberto R. and Varela, Francisco
1992The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Basis of Human Understanding. Boston: Shambhala.Google Scholar
Maturana, Humberto, Mpodozis, Jorge and Letelier, Juan C
1995 “Brain, language and the origin of human mental functions.” Biological Research 28: 15–26.Google Scholar
Mazokopaki, Katerina and Kugiumutzakis, Giannis
2009 “Infant rhythms: Expressions of musical companionship.” In Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship, Stephen Malloch and Colwyn Trevarthen (eds), 185–208. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McGilchrist, Iain
2009The Master and His Emissary: The Divided Brain and the Making of the Western World. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, David
2005Gesture and Thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meissner, Karin and Wittmann, Marc
2011 “Body signals, cardiac awareness, and the perception of time.” Biological Psychology 86 (3): 289–297. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Merker, Björn
2009 “Ritual foundations of human uniqueness.” In Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship, Stephen Malloch and Colwyn Trevarthen (eds), 45–60. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
2012 “The vocal learning constellation: Imitation, ritual culture, encephalization.” In Music, Language and Human Evolution, Nicholas Bannan (ed.), 215–262. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, Maurice
1961The Phenomenology of Perception. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Miall, David S. and Dissanayake, Ellen
2003 “The poetics of babytalk.” Human Nature 14 (4): 337–364. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Midgley, Mary
1994The Ethical Primate: Humans, Freedom and Morality. London: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mithen, Steven
2009 “The music instinct: The evolutionary basis of musicality.” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1169: 3–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagy, Emese
2011 “The newborn infant: A missing stage in developmental psychology.” Infant and Child Development 20: 3–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, Darcia, Panksepp, Jaak, Schore, Allan and Gleason, Tracy
2013Evolution, Early Experience and Human Development: From Research to Practice and Policy. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Oller, D. Kimbrough
2000Emergence of the Speech Capacity. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
2006 “Development and evolution in human vocal communication.” Biological Theory: Integrating Development, Evolution, and Cognition 1(4): 349–351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. Kimbrough and Griebel, Ulrike
2008 “Contextual flexibility in infant vocal development and the earliest steps in the evolution of language.” In Evolution of Communicative Flexibility: Complexity, Creativity, and Adaptability in Human and Animal Communication, D. Kimbrough Oller and Ulrike Griebel (eds), 141–168. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, Jaak
1998Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
2005 “Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans.” Consciousness and Cognition 14: 30–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, Jaak and Trevarthen, Colwyn
2009 “The neuroscience of emotion in music.” In Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship, Stephen Malloch and Colwyn Trevarthen (eds), 105–146. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Papoušek, Hanus
1996 “Musicality in infancy research: Biological and cultural origins of early musicality.” In Musical Beginnings: Origins and Development of Musical Competence, Irène Deliège and John Sloboda (eds), 37–55. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papoušek, Mechthild
1994 “Melodies in caregivers’ speech: A species specific guidance towards language.” Early Development and Parenting 3: 5–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1996 “Intuitive parenting: A hidden source of musical stimulation in infancy.” In Musical Beginnings: Origins and Development of Musical Competence, Irène Deliège and John Sloboda (eds), 88–112. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piontelli, Alessandra
2010Development of Normal Fetal Movements: The First 25 Weeks of Gestation. Heidelberg: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Porges, Stephen W. and Furman, Senta A
2011 “The early development of the autonomic nervous system provides a neural platform for social behavior: A polyvagal perspective.” Infant and Child Development 20: 106–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Powers, Niki and Trevarthen, Colwyn
2009 “Voices of shared emotion and meaning: Young infants and their mothers in Scotland and Japan.” In Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship, Stephen Malloch and Colwyn Trevarthen (eds), 209–240. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Reddy, Vasudevi
2008How Infants Know Minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
2011 “A gaze at grips with me.” In Joint Attention: New Developments in Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience, Axel Seemann (ed.), 137–158. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Reid, Thomas
1764An Inquiry into the Human Mind on the Principles of Common Sense. Printed for Andrew Millar, London, and Alexander Kincaid and John Bell, Edinburgh. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, Giacomo and Arbib, Michael A
1998 “Language within our grasp.” Trends in the Neurosciences 21: 188–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sacks, Oliver
2007Musicophilia: Tales of Music and the Brain. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Schögler, Ben and Trevarthen, Colwyn
2007 “To sing and dance together.” In On Being Moved: From Mirror Neurons to Empathy, Stein Bråten (ed.), 281–302. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schore, Allan N
2011The Science and Art of Psychotherapy. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Sebeok, Thomas A
1990Essays in Zoosemiotics [Monograph Series of the Toronto Semiotic Circle 5] Toronto: Toronto Semiotic Circle, Victoria College in the University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Sheets-Johnstone, Maxine
1990The Roots of Thinking. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
2012 “Fundamental and inherently related aspects of animation.” In Moving Ourselves, Moving Others: Motion and Emotion in Intersubjectivity, Consciousness and Language, Ad Foolen, Ulrike M. Lüdtke, Timothy P. Racine and Jordan Zlatev (eds), 29–56. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siegel, Daniel
2012The Developing Mind: How Relationships and the Brain Interact to Shape Who We Are. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Smith, Adam
1759Theory of Moral Sentiments. Edinburgh (Modern Edition: Oxford: Clarendon 1976) DOI: DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Solms, Mark and Panksepp, Jaak
2012 “The ‘Id’ knows more than the ‘Ego’ admits: Neuropsychoanalytic and primal consciousness perspectives on the interface between affective and cognitive neuroscience.” Brain Sciences 2: 147–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperry, Roger W
1982 “Some effects of disconnecting the cerebral hemispheres (Nobel lecture).” Science 217: 1223–1226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stern, Daniel N
2000The Interpersonal World of the Infant: A View from Psychoanalysis and Development Psychology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
2010Forms of Vitality: Exploring Dynamic Experience in Psychology, the Arts, Psychotherapy and Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stuart, Susan A.J
2013 “Enkinaesthesia and Reid’s natural kind of magic.” New Ideas in Psychology. An International Journal of Innovative Theory in Psychology (in press).Google Scholar
Stuart, Susan A.J. and Thibault, Paul J
2013 “Enkinaesthetic polyphony as the underpinning for first-order languaging.” Submitted to the Journal of Phenomenological Psychology.
Thibault, Paul J
2011 “First-order languaging dynamics and second-order language: The distributed language view.” Ecological Psychology 23 (3): 210–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael
1999The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
2003Constructing a Language: A Usage-based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, Colwyn
1977 “Descriptive analyses of infant communication behavior.” In Studies in Mother-Infant Interaction: The Loch Lomond Symposium, H. Rudolph Schaffer (ed.), 227–270. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
1979a “Communication and cooperation in early infancy. A description of primary intersubjectivity.” In Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication, Margaret Bullowa (ed.), 321–347. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
1979b “Instincts for human understanding and for cultural cooperation: Their development in infancy.” In Human Ethology, Mario von Cranach, K. Foppa, W. Lepenies and D. Ploog (eds). London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
1980 “The foundations of intersubjectivity: Development of interpersonal and cooperative understanding of infants.” In The Social Foundations of Language and Thought: Essays in Honor of Jerome S. Bruner, David Olson (ed.), 316–342. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
1982 “The primary motives for cooperative understanding.” In Social Cognition: Studies of the Development of Understanding, George Butterworth and Paul Light (eds), 77–109. Brighton: Harvester Press.Google Scholar
1983 “Interpersonal abilities of infants as generators for transmission of language and culture.” In The Behaviour of Human Infants, Alberto Oliverio and Michele Zapella (eds). New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1986 “Development of intersubjective motor control in infants.” In Motor Development in Children: Aspects of Coordination and Control, Michael G. Wade and H.T.A. Whiting (eds), 209–261. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhof. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1990a “Signs before speech.” In The Semiotic Web 1989, Thomas A. Sebeok and Jean Umiker-Sebeok (eds), 689–755. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1990b “Growth and education of the hemispheres.” In Brain Circuits and Functions of the Mind: Essays in Honour of Roger W. Sperry, Colwyn Trevarthen (ed.), 334–363. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
1992 “An infant’s motives for speaking and thinking in the culture.” In The Dialogical Alternative: Towards a Theory of Language and Mind (Festschrift for Ragnar Rommetveit), Astri H. Wold (ed.), 99–137. Oslo/Oxford: Scandinavian University Press/Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
1994 “Infant semiosis.” In Origins of Semiosis, Winfried Nöth (ed.), 219–252. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1995 “First impulses for communication: Negotiating meaning and moral sentiments with infants.” Journal of Contemporary Legal Issues 6: 373–407.Google Scholar
1996 “Lateral asymmetries in infancy: Implications for the development of the hemispheres.” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 20 (4): 571–586. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1998 “The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity.” In Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny, Stein Bråten (ed.), 15–46. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
1999 “Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication.” Musicae Scientiae Special Issue 1999–2000: 155–215.Google Scholar
2001 “The neurobiology of early communication: Intersubjective regulations in human brain development.” In Handbook on Brain and Behavior in Human Development, Alex F. Kalverboer and Albertus Gramsbergen (eds), 841–882. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
2002 “Origins of musical identity: Evidence from infancy for musical social awareness.” In Musical Identities, Raymond MacDonald, David J. Hargreaves and Dorothy Miell (eds), 21–38. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
2004a “Language development: Mechanisms in the brain.” In Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, (3rd ed.), CD-Rom. George Adelman and Barry H. Smith (eds). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
2004b “Brain development.” In Oxford Companion to the Mind, Richard L. Gregory (ed.), 116–127. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
2005a “Action and emotion in development of the human self, its sociability and cultural intelligence: Why infants have feelings like ours.” In Emotional Development, Jaqueline Nadel and Darwin Muir (eds), 61–91. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
2005b “First things first: Infants make good use of the sympathetic rhythm of imitation, without reason or language.” Journal of Child Psychotherapy 31 (1): 91–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2005c “Stepping away from the mirror: Pride and shame in adventures of companionship Reflections on the nature and emotional needs of infant intersubjectivity.” In Attachment and Bonding: A New Synthesis, C. Sue Carter, Lieselotte Ahnert, Klaus E. Grossman, Sarah B. Hrdy, Michael E. Lamb, Stephen W. Porges and Norbert Sachser (eds), 55–84. Dahlem Workshop Report 92. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
2009a “The intersubjective psychobiology of human meaning: Learning of culture depends on interest for co-operative practical work and affection for the joyful art of good company.” Psychoanalytic Dialogues 19 (5): 507–518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2009b “The functions of emotion in infancy: The regulation and communication of rhythm, sympathy, and meaning in human development.” In The Healing Power of Emotion: Affective Neuroscience, Development, and Clinical Practice, Diana Fosha, Daniel J. Siegel and Marion F. Solomon (eds), 55–85. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
2009c “Human biochronology: On the source and functions of ‘musicality’.” In Music That Works: Contributions of Biology, Neurophysiology, Psychology, Sociology, Medicine and Musicology, Roland Haas and Vera Brandes (eds), 221–265. Heidelberg: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2011a “The generation of human meaning: How shared experience grows in infancy.” In Joint Attention: New Developments in Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience, Axel Seemann (ed.), 73–135. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
2011b “What is it like to be a person who knows nothing? Defining the active intersubjective mind of a newborn human being.” Infant and Child Development 20 (1): 119–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2011c “Innate moral feelings, moral laws and cooperative cultural practice.” In Moral Behavior and Free Will: A Neurobiological and Philosophical Approach, Juan J. Sanguineti, Alberto Acerbi and José A. Lombo (eds), 377–411. Morolo: IF Press.Google Scholar
2012a “The infant’s voice grows in intimate dialogue: How musicality of expression inspires shared meaning.” In Dialogic Formations: Investigations into the Origins of the Dialogical Self, Marie-Cécile Bertau, Miguel M. Gonçalves and Peter T.F. Raggatt (eds), 3–40. Charlotte, NC: Information Age.Google Scholar
2012b “Epilogue: Natural sources of meaning in human sympathetic vitality.” In Moving Ourselves, Moving Others: Motion and Emotion in Intersubjectivity, Consciousness and Language, Ad Foolen, Ulrike M. Lüdtke, Timothy P. Racine and Jordan Zlatev (eds), 451–483. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2013 “Born for art, and the joyful companionship of fiction.” In Evolution, Early Experience and Human Development: From Research to Practice and Policy, Darcia Narvaez, Jaak Panksepp, Allan Schore and Tracy Gleason (eds), 202–218. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, Colwyn and Aitken, Kenneth J
1994 “Brain development, infant communication, and empathy disorders: Intrinsic factors in child mental health.” Development and Psychopathology 6: 599–635. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, Colwyn, Aitken, Kenneth J., Vandekerckhove, Marie, Delafield-Butt, Jonathan and Nagy, Emese
2006 “Collaborative regulations of vitality in early childhood: Stress in intimate relationships and postnatal psychopathology.” In Developmental Psychopathology, Volume 2, Developmental Neuroscience, Dante Cicchetti and David J. Cohen (eds), 65–126. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, Colwyn and Delafield-Butt, Jonathan
2013a “Biology of shared experience and language development: Regulations for the intersubjective life of narratives.” In The Infant Mind: Origins of the Social Brain, Maria Legerstee, David Haley and Marc Bornstein (eds), 167–199. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
2013b “Autism as a developmental disorder in intentional movement and affective engagement.” In Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, Dr. Elizabeth B. Torres, Robert W. Isenhower, Caroline Whyatt and Anne M. Donnellan (eds). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, Colwyn, Delafield-Butt, Jonathan and Schögler, Ben
2011 “Psychobiology of musical gesture: Innate rhythm, harmony and melody in movements of narration.” In New Perspectives on Music and Gesture (SEMPRE studies in the psychology of music), Anthony Gritten and Elaine King (eds), 11–43. Farnham: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, Colwyn and Hubley, Penelope
1978 “Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year.” In Action, Gesture and Symbol: The Emergence of Language, Andrew Lock (ed.), 183–229. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, Colwyn and Malloch, Stephen
2002 “Musicality and music before three: Human vitality and invention shared with pride.” Zero to Three 23 (1): 10–18.Google Scholar
Turner, Mark
1996The Literary Mind: The Origins of Thought and Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tzourio-Mazoyer, Nathalie, De Schonen, Scania, Crivello, Fabrice, Reutter, Bryan, Aujard, Yannick and Mazoyer, Bernard
2002 “Neural correlates of woman face processing by 2-month-old infants.” Neuroimage 15: 454–461. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vandekerckhove, Marie and Panksepp, Jaak
2011 “The neural evolution of consciousness: From anoetic affective experiences to noetic and autonoetic cognitive awareness.” Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (9): 2017–2025. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, Lev S
1978Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Edited by Michael Cole, Vera Steiner, Sylvia Scribner and Ellen Souberman. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Whitehead, Alfred N
1929The Aims of Education and Other Essays. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Wittmann, Marc
2009 “The inner experience of time.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences 364: 1955–1967. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Busch, Brigitta
2020. Discourse, Emotions and Embodiment. In The Cambridge Handbook of Discourse Studies,  pp. 327 ff. DOI logo
Anna De Fina & Alexandra Georgakopoulou
2020. The Cambridge Handbook of Discourse Studies, DOI logo
Lüdtke, Ulrike M. & Hanna Ehlert
2023. Linguistic Feeling: A Relational Approach Incorporating Epistemology, Theories of Language, and Human-Machine Interaction. In Emotions, Metacognition, and the Intuition of Language Normativity,  pp. 317 ff. DOI logo
Niikuni, Keiyu, Ming Wang, Michiru Makuuchi, Masatoshi Koizumi & Sachiko Kiyama
2022. Pupil Dilation Reflects Emotional Arousal Via Poetic Language. Perceptual and Motor Skills 129:6  pp. 1691 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.