A long-standing divide between Icelandic and German in the literature takes for granted that there are
non-nominative subjects in Icelandic, while corresponding arguments in German have been analyzed as objects (Zaenen et al. 1985; Sigurðsson 1989). This is based on two
differences between these languages: (a) differences with regard to control and conjunction reduction and (b) apparent subject
behavior of the nominative in dat-nom constructions in German. This article focuses on the latter, introducing into the discussion
the concept of alternating predicates, that is, dat-nom predicates that systematically alternate between two diametrically-opposed
argument structure constructions, dat-nom and nom-dat. A comparison between Icelandic and German shows that Icelandic dat-nom
predicates are of two types, a non-alternating líka type and an alternating falla í geð type,
whereas German seems to exhibit only the alternating type. On this assumption, the apparent subject behavior of the nominative in
German is easily explained, since such occurrences in fact involve the nom-dat construction and not the dat-nom construction.
Therefore, the subject behavior of the nominative in nom-dat constructions does not invalidate a subject analysis of the dative in
dat-nom constructions in German. The analysis is couched in the framework of construction grammar.
Allen, C. L. (1995). Case marking and reanalysis: Grammatical relations from Old to Early Modern English. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Allen, K., Pereira, F., Botvinick, M., & Goldberg, A. E. (2012). Distinguishing grammatical constructions with fMRI pattern analysis. Brain and Language, 1231, 174–182.
Anderson, S. R. (1976). On the notion of subject in ergative languages. In: Ch. Li (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp. 1–23). New York: Academic Press.
Andrews, A. (1976). The VP complement analysis in Modern Icelandic. North Eastern Linguistic Society, 61, 1–21.
Barðdal, J. (1998). Argument structure, syntactic structure and morphological case of the impersonal construction in the history of Scandinavian. Scripta Islandica, 491, 21–33.
Barðdal, J. (1999). The dual nature of Icelandic psych-verbs. Working Papers in Scandinavian Syntax, 641, 78–101.
Barðdal, J. (2000). The subject is nominative! On obsolete axioms and their deep-rootedness. In: C.-E. Lindberg & S. Nordahl Lund (Eds.), 17th Scandinavian Conference of Linguistics (pp. 93–117). Odense: Institute of Language and Communication.
Barðdal, J. (2001a). The perplexity of dat-nom verbs in Icelandic. Nordic Journal of Linguistics, 241, 47–70.
Barðdal, J. (2001b). The role of thematic roles in constructions? Evidence from the Icelandic inchoative. In: A. Holmer, J.-O. Svantesson, & Å. Viberg (Eds.), Proceedings of the 18th Scandinavian Conference of Linguistics 2000 (pp. 127–137). Lund: Department of Linguistics.
Barðdal, J. (2002). Oblique subjects in Icelandic and German. Working Papers in Scandinavian Syntax, 701, 61–99.
Barðdal, J. (2004). The semantics of the impersonal construction in Icelandic, German and Faroese. In: W. Abraham (Ed.), Focus on Germanic Typology (pp. 105–137). Berlin: Akademie Verlag.
Barðdal, J. (2006). Construction-specific properties of syntactic subjects in Icelandic and German. Cognitive Linguistics, 17(1), 39–106.
Barðdal, J. (2011a). Alternating dat-nom/nom-dat verbs in a Germanic context. Talk delivered at the
33rd Annual Conference of the Deutsche Gesellschaft für Sprachwissenschaft
, Göttingen, February 23–25.
Barðdal, J. (2011b). The rise of dative substitution in the history of Icelandic: A diachronic construction grammar approach. Lingua, 121(1), 60–79.
Barðdal, J., & Eythórsson, Th. (2003). Icelandic vs. German: Oblique subjects, agreement and expletives. Chicago Linguistics Society, 39(1), 775–773.
Barðdal, J., & Eythórsson, Th. (2012). “Hungering and lusting for women and fleshly delicacies”: Reconstructing grammatical relations for Proto-Germanic. Transactions of the Philological Society, 110(3), 363–393.
Barðdal, J., & Eythórsson, Th. (2018). What is a subject? The nature and validity of subject tests. In: J. Barðdal, N. Pat-El, & S. M. Carey (Eds.), Non-canonically case-marked subjects: The Reykjavík – Eyjafjallajökull Papers (pp. 257–273). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Barðdal, J., Kristoffersen, K. E., & Sveen, A. (2011). West Scandinavian ditransitives as a family of constructions: With a special attention to the Norwegian V-REFL-NP construction. Linguistics, 49(1), 53–104.
Barlow, M., & Kemmer, S. (Eds). (2000). Usage-based models of language. Stanford: CSLI Publications.
Barnes, M. (1986). Subject, nominative, and oblique case in Faroese. Scripta Islandica, 381, 3–35.
Bayer, J. (2004). Non-nominative subjects in comparison. In: P. Bhaskararao & K. V. Subbarao (Eds.), Non-nominative subjects 11 (pp. 31–58). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Bayer, J., Bader, M., & Meng, M. (2001). Morphological underspecification meets oblique case: Syntactic and processing effects in German. Lingua, 1111, 465–514.
Belletti, A., & Rizzi, L. (1988). Psych-verbs and theta-theory. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 61, 291–352.
Bernódusson, H. (1982). Ópersónulegar setningar [Impersonal sentences]. University of Iceland Master’s Thesis.
Bybee, J. L. (2013). Usage-based theory and exemplar representations of constructions. In: T. Hoffmann & G. Trousdale (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar (pp. 49–69). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cole, P., Harbert, W., Hermon, G., & Sridhar, S. N. (1980). The aquisition of subjecthood. Language, 56(4), 719–743.
Comrie, B. (1973). The ergative: Variations on a theme. Lingua, 321, 239–253.
Croft, W. (1998). Event structure in argument linking. In: M. Butt & W. Geuder (Eds.), The projection of arguments: Lexical and compositional factors (pp. 1–43). Stanford: CSLI.
Croft, W. (2001). Radical Construction Grammar: Syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Croft, W. (2003). Lexical rules vs. constructions: A false dichotomy. In: H. Cuyckens, Th. Berg, R. Dirven, & K.-U. Panther (Eds.), Motivations in language: Studies in honour of Günter Radden (pp. 49–68). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Croft, W. (2012). Verbs: Aspect and causal structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Croft, W., & Cruse, D. A. (2004). Cognitive linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Diessel, H. (2015). Usage-based construction grammar. In: E. Dąbrowska & D. Divjak (Ed.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 295–321). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Eythórsson, Th., & Barðdal, J. (2005). Oblique subjects: A common Germanic inheritance. Language, 81(4), 824–881.
Falk, C. (1997). Fornsvenska upplevarverb [Old Swedish experiencer verbs]. Lund: Lund University Press.
Fanselow, G. (2002). Quirky subjects and other specifiers. In: I. Kaufmann & B. Stiebels (Eds.), More than words: A festschrift for Dieter Wunderlich (pp. 227–250). Berlin: Akademie Verlag.
Fedriani, Ch. (2014). Experiential constructions in Latin: A synchronic and diachronic study. Leiden: Brill.
Fillmore, Ch. J., Kay, P., & O’Connor, M. K. (1988). Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: The case of let alone. Language, 641, 501–538.
Fillmore, Ch. J., Lee-Goldman, R., & Rhodes, R. (2012). The FrameNet constructicon. In: I. A. Sag & H. C. Boas (Eds.), Sign-based construction grammar (pp. 283–299). Stanford: CSLI Publications.
Fischer, O. (1990). Syntactic change and causation: Developments in infinitival constructions in English. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Amsterdam.
Fischer, O., & van der Leek, F. C. (1983). The demise of the Old English impersonal construction. Journal of Linguistics, 19(2), 337–368.
Fischer, S., & Blaszczak, J. (2001). Diachronic perspective of quirky subjects. In: P. Bhaskararao (Ed.), International Symposium on “Non-Nominative Subjects”, organized by ILCAA, Tokyo, 18–21December 2001. Working Papers, 42–56.
Fried, M., & Östman, J.-O. (2005). Construction grammar and spoken language: The case of pragmatic particles. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(11), 1752–1778.
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument atructure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Goldberg, A. E., & Bencini, G. M. L. (2005). Support from language processing for a constructional approach to grammar. In: A. Tyler, M. Takada, Y. Kim, & D. Marinova (Eds.), Language in use: Cognitive and discourse perspectives on language and language learning (pp. 3–18). Georgetown University Round Table on Languages and Linguistics.
Haider, H. (2005). How to turn German into Icelandic – and derive the OV–VO contrast. Journal of Comparative Germanic Syntax, 81, 1–53.
Höhle, T. N. (1978). Lexikalistische Syntax: die Aktiv-Passiv-Relation und andere Infinitivstrukturen im Deutschen. Tübingen: Niemeyer.
Hopper, P. J. (1987). Emergent grammar. Berkeley Linguistics Society, 131, 139–157.
Hyams, N., & Sigurjónsdóttir, S. (1990). The development of ‘long-distance anaphora’: A cross-linguistic comparison with special reference to Icelandic. Language Acquisition, 11, 57–93.
Jackendoff, R. (1997). Twistin’ the night away. Language, 731, 534–559.
Jónsson, J. G. (1996). Clausal architecture and case in Icelandic. University of Massachusetts, Amherst, Doctoral Dissertation.
Jónsson, J. G. (1997–98). Sagnir með aukafallsfrumlagi [Verbs with quirky subjects]. Íslenskt mál og almenn málfræði, 19–201, 11–43.
Jónsson, J. G. (2001). Oblique subject predicates in Icelandic. Unpublished ms., University of Iceland.
Kay, P., & Fillmore, Ch. J. (1999). Grammatical constructions and linguistic generalizations: The ‘What’s X doing Y?‘ construction. Language, 751, 1–33.
Keenan, E. L. (1976). Towards a universal definition of subject. In Ch. N. Li (Ed.), Subject and Topic (pp. 303–333). New York: Academic Press.
Keller, F., Lapata, M., & Ourioupina, O. (2002). Using the web to overcome data sparseness. In: J. Hajič & Y. Matsumoto (Eds.), Proceedings of the Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing, (pp. 230–237). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania & the Association for Computational Linguistics.
Kiss, T. (2003). Die Genese der Ausnahmeanapher. In: L. Gunkel, G. Müller, & G. Zifonun (Eds.), Arbeiten zur Reflexivierung (pp. 157–188). Tübingen: Niemeyer.
Kristoffersen, K. E. (1996). Infinitival phrases in Old Norse: Aspects of their syntax and semantics. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Oslo.
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Langacker, R. W. (1991). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Lenerz, J. (1977). Zur Abfolge nominaler Satzglieder im Deutschen. Tübingen: Günter Narr.
MacWhinney, B. (2001). Emergentist approaches to language. In: J. Bybee & P. Hopper (Eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure (pp. 449–470). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Maling, J. (1986). Clause-bounded examples in Modern Icelandic. In: L. Helland & K. Koch Christensen (Eds.), Topics in Scandinavian syntax (pp. 53–63). Dordrecht: Reidel. [Also published in J. Maling & A. Zaenen (Eds.), Modern Icelandic syntax (pp. 277–287). Syntax and Semantics 24. San Diego: Academic Press.]
Masica, C. P. (1976). Defining a linguistic area: South Asia. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Michaelis, L. A., & Ruppenhofer, J. (2001). Beyond alternations: A construction-based account of the applicative construction in German. Stanford: CSLI Publications.
Moore, J., & Perlmutter, D. M. (2000). What does it take to be a dative Subject?Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 181, 373–416.
Müller, S. (2012). On the copula, specificational constructions, and type shifting. Ms. Freie Universitet Berlin.
Nedjalkov, V. P. (1976). Kausativkonstruktionen. Tübingen: Gunter Narr.
Platzack, Ch. (1999). The subject of Icelandic psych-verbs: A minimalistic account. Working Papers in Scandinavian Syntax, 641, 103–116.
Primus, B. (1994). Grammatik und Performanz: Faktoren der Wortstellungsvariation im Mittelfeld. Sprache und Pragmatik, 321, 39–86.
Primus, B. (2012). Semantische Rollen. Heidelberg: Carl Winter.
Reis, M. (1973). Is there a rule of subject-to-object-raising in German?Chicago Linguistic Society, 91, 519–529.
Reis, M. (1976). Reflexivierung in deutschen A.c.I.-konstruktionen: Ein transformationsgrammatisches Dilemma. Papiere zur Linguistik, 91, 5–82.
Rögnvaldsson, E. (1991). Quirky subjects in Old Icelandic. In: H. Á. Sigurðsson (Ed.), Papers from the Twelfth Scandinavian Conference of Linguistics (pp. 369–378). Reykjavík: Institute of Linguistics, University of Iceland.
Rögnvaldsson, E. (1996). Frumlag og fall að fornu [Subject and case in Old Icelandic]. Íslenskt mál, 181, 37–69.
Rott, J. A. (2013). Syntactic prominence in Icelandic experiencer arguments: Quirky subjects vs. dative objects. STUF – Language Typology and Universals, 66(2), 91–111.
Sasse, H.-J. (1978). Subjekt und Ergativ: Zur pragmatischen Grundlage primärer grammatischer Relationen. Folia Linguistica, 121, 219–252.
Sigurðsson, H. Á. (1989). Verbal syntax and case in Icelandic. Lund University Doctoral dissertation.
Sigurðsson, H. Á. (1990–91). Beygingarsamræmi [Agreement]. Íslenskt mál, 12–131, 31–77.
Sigurðsson, H. Á. (2006a). The nominative puzzle and the low nominative hypothesis. Linguistic Inquiry, 371, 289–308.
Sigurðsson, H. Á. (2006b). Agree in syntax, agreement in signs. In: C. Boeckx (Ed.), Agreement systems (pp. 201–237). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Stepanov, A. (2003). On the ‘quirky’ difference Icelandic vs. German: A note of doubt. Working Papers in Scandinavian Syntax, 711, 1–32.
Struckmeier, V. (2016). Scrambling in German is driven by prosody and semantics. In: K.-M. Kim, P. Umbal, T. Block, Q. Chan, T. Cheng, K. Finney, M. Katz, S. Nickel-Thompson, & L. Shorten (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp. 381–389). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.
Talmy, L. (1985). Force dynamics in language and thought. Chicago Linguistic Society, 211, 293–337.
Talmy, L. (1988). Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science, 12(1), 49–100.
Thráinsson, H. (1979). On complementation in Icelandic [Outstanding Dissertations in Linguistics]. New York: Garland.
Thráinsson, H. (2007). The syntax of Icelandic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Thráinsson, H., Petersen, H. P., Jacobsen, J. í L., & Hansen, Z. S. (2012). Faroese: An overview and reference grammar. Second edition. Tórshavn: Føroya Fróðskaparfelag.
Wood, J. (2011). Icelandic let-causatives and case. Working Papers in Scandinavian Syntax, 971, 1–44.
Wood, J., & Sigurðsson, H. Á. (2014). Let-causatives and (a)symmetric DAT-NOM constructions. Syntax, 17(3), 269–298.
Wunderlich, D. (2008). The force of lexical case: German and Icelandic compared. In: K. Hanson & Sh. Inkelas (Eds.), The nature of the word: Essays in honor of Paul Kiparsky (pp. 587–620). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wurmbrand, S. (2003). Infinitives: Restructuring and clause structure. Berlin: Walter De Gruyter.
Zaenen, A., Maling, J., & Thráinsson, H. (1985). Case and grammatical functions: The Icelandic passive. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 31, 441–483.
Cited by (4)
Cited by four other publications
Somers, Joren, Gard B. Jenset & Jóhanna Barðdal
2024. Argument structure constructions in competition: The Dat-Nom/Nom-Dat alternation in Icelandic. Nordic Journal of Linguistics► pp. 1 ff.
Vázquez-González, Juan G.
2024. Updating Old English Dative–Genitives: A Diachronic Construction Grammar Account. Languages 9:6 ► pp. 213 ff.
Ilioaia, Mihaela & Marleen Van Peteghem
2021. Dative experiencers with nominal predicates in Romanian: a synchronic and diachronic study. Folia Linguistica 55:s42-s2 ► pp. 255 ff.
Barđdal, Jóhanna, Leonid Kulikov, Roland Pooth & Peter Alexander Kerkhof
2020. Oblique anticausatives: A morphosyntactic isogloss in Indo-European. Poznan Studies in Contemporary Linguistics 56:3 ► pp. 413 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 1 august 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.