Article published in:
Language Structure and Environment: Social, cultural, and natural factors
Edited by Rik De Busser and Randy J. LaPolla
[Cognitive Linguistic Studies in Cultural Contexts 6] 2015
► pp. 289316
References

Bibliography

Borges, J.L.
(1964) Labyrinths: Selected stories & other writings. New York, NY: New Directions Publishing.
Brody, M.
(1998) The minimalist program and a perfect syntax: A critical notice of Noam Chomsky’s The Minimalist Program. Mind and Language, 13(2), 205–214. doi: 10.1111/1468‑0017.00074
Calvet, J.-L.
(2006) Towards an ecology of world languages. Cambridge: Polity.
Cassirer, E.
(1962) An essay on man: An introduction to a philosophy of human culture. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Chater, N., Reali, F., & Christiansen, M.H.
(2009) Restrictions on biological adaptation in language evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(4), 1015–1020. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0807191106
Christiansen, M.H., & Chater, N.
(2008) Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(5), 489–509. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X08004998
Croft, W.
(2002) Typology and universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511840579
Crowley, T., & Bowern, C.
(2010) An introduction to historical linguistics. New York: Oxford University Press.
Dahl, Ö.
(2004) The growth and maintenance of linguistic complexity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/slcs.71
Dale, R., & Lupyan, G.
(2012) Understanding the origins of morphological diversity: The linguistic niche hypothesis. Advances in Complex Systems, 15(3), 1150017–1–1150017–16. doi: 10.1142/S0219525911500172
Davies, M.
(2013, April). Corpus of global web-based English: 1.9 billion words from speakers in 20 countries (GloWbE).Brighman Young University. Online Corpus. Retrieved15 September 2013fromcorpus2.byu.edu/​glowbe/​
Deacon, T.W.
(1997) The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the Brain. London: Penguin.
Denny, J.P.
(1978) Locating the universals in lexical systems for spatial deixis. In D.F.Farkas, W.M.Jacobsen, & K.W.Todrys(Eds.), Papers from the Parasession on the Lexicon (pp. 71–84). Chicago, IL: Chicago Linguistic Society.
(1982) Semantics of the Inuktitut (Eskimo) spatial deictics. International Journal of American Linguistics, 48(4), 359–384. doi: 10.1086/465747
Devescovi, A., Caselli, M.C., Marchione, D., Pasqualetti, P., Reilly, J., & Bates, E.
(2005) A crosslinguistic study of the relationship between grammar and lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 32(4), 759–786. doi: 10.1017/S0305000905007105
Dryer, M.S., & Haspelmath, M.
(Eds.) (2011) The world atlas of language structures online. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library. Retrieved fromwals.info/​
Ellefson, M.R., & Christiansen, M.H.
(2000) Subjacency constraints without universal grammar: Evidence from artificial language learning and connectionist modeling. In L.R.Gleitman & A.K.Joshi(Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-second Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 645–650). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ember, C.R., & Ember, M.
(2007) Climate, econiche, and sexuality: Influences on sonority in language. American Anthropologist, 109(1), 180–185. doi: 10.1525/aa.2007.109.1.180
Enfield, N.J.
(Ed.) (2004) Ethnosyntax: Explorations in grammar and culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199266500.001.0001
Evans, N.
(2003) Bininj Gun-Wok: A Pan-Dialectal Grammar of Mayali, Kunwinjku and Kune (2 Volumes). Canberra: Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, Australian National University.
Evans, N., & Levinson, S.C.
(2009) The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(05), 429–448. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999094X
Everett, D.L.
(2005) Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: Another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology, 46(4), 621–646. doi: 10.1086/431525
(2009) Pirahã culture and grammar: A response to some criticisms. Language, 85(2), 1–38. doi: 10.1353/lan.0.0092
Frigo, L., & McDonald, J.L.
(1998) Properties of phonological markers that affect the acquisition of gender-like subclasses. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(2), 218–245. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2569
Gathercole, S.E., Pickering, S.J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H.
(2004) The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 177–190. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.40.2.177
Gil, D.
(2009) How much grammar does it take to sail a boat? In G.Sampson, D.Gil, & P.Trudgill(Eds.), Language complexity as an evolving variable (pp. 19–33). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gordon, R.G.
(Ed.) (2005) Ethnologue: Languages of the world, Fifteenth Edition. Dallas, TX: SIL International. Retrieved fromarchive.ethnologue.com/​15/​
Hart, B., & Risley, T.R.
(1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Hockett, C.F.
(1966) The problem of universals in language. In J.H.Greenberg(Ed.), Universals of language, 2nd Edition, Volume 2 (pp. 1–29). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hoff, E.
(2003) The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74(5), 1368–1378. doi: 10.1111/1467‑8624.00612
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V.A., & Fernald, A.
(2008) Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11(6), F31–F39. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2008.00768.x
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S.
(2002) Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45(3), 337–374. doi: 10.1016/S0010‑0285(02)00500‑5
Imaizumi, H., Kunimatsu, S., & Isei, T.
(2000) Sound propagation and speech transmission in a branching underground tunnel. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 108(2), 632–642. doi: 10.1121/1.429595
Jelinek, E., & Demers, R.A.
(1994) Predicates and pronominal arguments in Straits Salish. Language, 70(4), 697–736. doi: 10.2307/416325
Jespersen, O.
(1922) Language: Its nature, development and origin. London: George Allan & Unwin.
Johnston, J.R., & Wong, M.-Y.
A (2002) Cultural differences in beliefs and practices concerning talk to children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(5), 916–926. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2002/074)
Keller, R.
(1994) On language change: The invisible hand in language. (B.Nerlich, Trans.). London: Routledge.
Kinzler, K.D., Shutts, K., DeJesus, J., & Spelke, E.S.
(2009) Accent trumps race in guiding children’s social preferences. Social Cognition, 27(4), 623–634. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.4.623
Kuczaj, S.A.
(1979) Evidence for a language learning strategy: On the relative ease of acquisition of prefixes and suffixes. Child Development, 50(1), 1–13. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.1979.tb02972.x
Lasnik, H.
(2002) The minimalist program in syntax. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(10), 432–437. doi: 10.1016/S1364‑6613(02)01977‑0
Levinson, S.C.
(2003) Language and mind: Let’s get the issues straight! In D.Gentner & S.Goldin-Meadow(Eds.), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought (pp. 25–46). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Levinson, S.C., & Wilkins, D.P.
(2006) Grammars of space: Explorations in cognitive diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511486753
Lewis, R.L.
(1996) Interference in short-term memory: The magical number two (or three) in sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(1), 93–115. doi: 10.1007/BF01708421
Lupyan, G., & Dale, R.
(2010) Language structure is partly determined by social structure. PLoS ONE, 5(1), e8559. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008559
MacWhinney, B.
(1983) Miniature linguistic systems as tests of the use of universal operating principles in second-language learning by children and adults. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 12(5), 467–478. doi: 10.1007/BF01068027
Maddieson, I., Bhattacharya, T., Smith, D.E., & Croft, W.
(2011) Geographical distribution of phonological complexity. Linguistic Typology, 15(2), 267–279. doi: 10.1515/lity.2011.020
McWhorter, J.H.
(2001) The world’s simplest grammars are creole grammars. Linguistic Typology, 5(2-3), 125–166. doi: 10.1515/lity.2001.001
(2002) What happened to English?Diachronica, 19(2), 217–272. doi: 10.1075/dia.19.2.02wha
(2007) Language interrupted: Signs of non-native acquisition in standard language grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195309805.001.0001
Meyer, J.
(2004) Bioacoustics of human whistled languages: An alternative approach to the cognitive processes of language. Anais Da Academia Brasileira de Ciências, 76(2), 406–412. doi: 10.1590/S0001‑37652004000200033
Mithun, M.
(1984) How to avoid subordination. InProceedings of the Tenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 493–509). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.
Monaghan, P., Christiansen, M.H., & Fitneva, S.A.
(2011) The arbitrariness of the sign: Learning advantages from the structure of the vocabulary. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(3), 325–347. doi: 10.1037/a0022924
Nettle, D.
(1996) Language diversity in West Africa: An ecological approach. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 15(4), 403–438. doi: 10.1006/jaar.1996.0015
(1998a) Coevolution of phonology and the lexicon in twelve languages of West Africa. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 5(3), 240–245. doi: 10.1080/09296179808590132
(1998b) Explaining global patterns of language diversity. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 17(4), 354–374. doi: 10.1006/jaar.1998.0328
Nowak, M.A., Komarova, N.L., & Niyogi, P.
(2001) Evolution of universal grammar. Science, 291(5501), 114–118. doi: 10.1126/science.291.5501.114
Perkins, R.D.
(1992) Deixis, grammar, and culture. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/tsl.24
Piatelli-Palmarini, M., & Uriagereka, J.
(2004) The immune syntax: The evolution of the language virus. In L.Jenkins(Ed.), Variation and Universals in biolinguistics (pp. 341–377). Oxford: Elsevier.
Pinker, S.
(1994) The language instinct. New York, NY: Harper Collins. doi: 10.1037/e412952005‑009
Pinker, S., & Bloom, P.
(1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13(4), 707–784. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00081061
Premack, D.
(1986) Gavagai! or the future history of the animal language controversy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Richman, A.L., Miller, P.M., & LeVine, R.A.
(1992) Cultural and educational variations in maternal responsiveness. Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 614–621. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.28.4.614
Sampson, G., Gil, D., & Trudgill, P.
(2009) Language complexity as an evolving variable. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sapir, E.
(1921) Language: An introduction to the study of speech. New York: Hartcourt, Brace & World.
Slobin, D.I.
(1979) Psycholinguistics, 2nd Edition. Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman & Co.
(1985) Crosslinguistic evidence for the language making capacity. In D.I.Slobin(Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Volume 2: Theoretical issues (pp. 1157–1256). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
(1997) The universal, the typological, and the particular in acquisition. In E.Wanner & L.R.Gleitman(Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 1–39). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Slobin, D.I., & Bever, T.G.
(1982) Children use canonical sentence schemas: A crosslinguistic study of word order and inflections. Cognition, 12(3), 229–265. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0277(82)90033‑6
St. Clair, M.C., Monaghan, P., & Ramscar, M.
(2009) Relationships between language structure and language learning: The suffixing preference and drammatical categorization. Cognitive Science, 33(7), 1317–1329. doi: 10.1111/j.1551‑6709.2009.01065.x
Tamis-LeMonda, C.S., Song, L., Leavell, A.S., Kahana-Kalman, R., & Yoshikawa, H.
(2012) Ethnic differences in mother-infant language and gestural communications are associated with specific skills in infants: Mother-infant communications. Developmental Science, 15(3), 384–397. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2012.01136.x
Thompson, L.C.
(1987) A vietnamese reference grammar. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.
Thurston, W.R.
(1989) How exoteric languages build a lexicon: Esoterogeny in West New Britain. In R.Harlow & R.Cooper(Eds.), Papers from the Fifth International Conference on Austronesian Linguistics (pp. 555–579). Auckland: Linguistic Society of New Zealand.
Trudgill, P.
(1988) On the role of dialect contact and interdialect in linguistic change. In J.Fisiak(Ed.), Historical Dialectology. Regional and social (pp. 547–563). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. doi: 10.1515/9783110848137.547
(1989) Contact and isolation in linguistic change. In L.E.Breivik & E.H.Jahr(Eds.), Language change: Contributions to the study of its causes (pp.227–238). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. doi: 10.1515/9783110853063.227
(1993) Dialect typology: Phonological aspects. In G.Aurrekoetxea & X.Videgain(Eds.), Nazioarteko Dialektologia Blitzarra: Agiriak / Actas del congreso internacional de dialectología (pp. 659–666). Bilbao: Euskaltzainda.
(2001a) Contact and simplification: Historical baggage and directionality in linguistic change. Linguistic Typology, 5(2-3), 371–374.
(2001b) Sociolinguistics: An introduction to language and society, Fourth Edition. London: Penguin.
Vogt, P., & Mastin, J.D.
(2013) Rural and urban differences in language socialization and early vocabulary development in Mozambique. In M.Knauff, M.Pauen, M.Sebanz, & I.Wachsmuth(Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3787–3792). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Weiner, J.
(1995) The beak of the Finch: A story of evolution in our time. New York, NY: Vintage.
White, D.R.
(2007) Standard cross-cultural sample free distribution site (UC Irvine): Comparative research, world cultural comparison, courses and workshops. Retrieved31 March 2009fromeclectic.ss.uci.edu/​~drwhite/​courses/​index.html
Wray, A., & Grace, G.W.
(2007) The consequences of talking to strangers: Evolutionary corollaries of socio-cultural influences on linguistic form. Lingua, 117(3), 543–578. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2005.05.005