Article published in:
Language Structure and Environment: Social, cultural, and natural factors
Edited by Rik De Busser and Randy J. LaPolla
[Cognitive Linguistic Studies in Cultural Contexts 6] 2015
► pp. 289316
Bibliography

Bibliography

Borges, J.L.
(1964) Labyrinths: Selected stories & other writings. New York, NY: New Directions Publishing.
Brody, M.
(1998) The minimalist program and a perfect syntax: A critical notice of Noam Chomsky’s The Minimalist Program. Mind and Language, 13(2), 205–214. Crossref link
Calvet, J.-L.
(2006) Towards an ecology of world languages. Cambridge: Polity.
Cassirer, E.
(1962) An essay on man: An introduction to a philosophy of human culture. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Chater, N., Reali, F., & Christiansen, M.H.
(2009) Restrictions on biological adaptation in language evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(4), 1015–1020. Crossref link
Christiansen, M.H., & Chater, N.
(2008) Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(5), 489–509. Crossref link
Croft, W.
(2002) Typology and universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref link
Crowley, T., & Bowern, C.
(2010) An introduction to historical linguistics. New York: Oxford University Press.
Dahl, Ö.
(2004) The growth and maintenance of linguistic complexity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Crossref link
Dale, R., & Lupyan, G.
(2012) Understanding the origins of morphological diversity: The linguistic niche hypothesis. Advances in Complex Systems, 15(3), 1150017–1–1150017–16. Crossref link
Davies, M.
(2013, April). Corpus of global web-based English: 1.9 billion words from speakers in 20 countries (GloWbE). Brighman Young University. Online Corpus. Retrieved 15 September 2013 from http://​corpus2​.byu​.edu​/glowbe/
Deacon, T.W.
(1997) The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the Brain. London: Penguin.
Denny, J.P.
(1978) Locating the universals in lexical systems for spatial deixis. In D.F. Farkas, W.M. Jacobsen, & K.W. Todrys (Eds.), Papers from the Parasession on the Lexicon (pp. 71–84). Chicago, IL: Chicago Linguistic Society.
(1982) Semantics of the Inuktitut (Eskimo) spatial deictics. International Journal of American Linguistics, 48(4), 359–384. Crossref link
Devescovi, A., Caselli, M.C., Marchione, D., Pasqualetti, P., Reilly, J., & Bates, E.
(2005) A crosslinguistic study of the relationship between grammar and lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 32(4), 759–786. Crossref link
Dryer, M.S., & Haspelmath, M.
(Eds.) (2011) The world atlas of language structures online. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library. Retrieved from http://​wals​.info/
Ellefson, M.R., & Christiansen, M.H.
(2000) Subjacency constraints without universal grammar: Evidence from artificial language learning and connectionist modeling. In L.R. Gleitman & A.K. Joshi (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-second Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 645–650). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ember, C.R., & Ember, M.
(2007) Climate, econiche, and sexuality: Influences on sonority in language. American Anthropologist, 109(1), 180–185. Crossref link
Enfield, N.J.
(Ed.) (2004) Ethnosyntax: Explorations in grammar and culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Crossref link
Evans, N.
(2003) Bininj Gun-Wok: A Pan-Dialectal Grammar of Mayali, Kunwinjku and Kune (2 Volumes). Canberra: Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, Australian National University.
Evans, N., & Levinson, S.C.
(2009) The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(05), 429–448. Crossref link
Everett, D.L.
(2005) Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: Another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology, 46(4), 621–646. Crossref link
(2009) Pirahã culture and grammar: A response to some criticisms. Language, 85(2), 1–38. Crossref link
Frigo, L., & McDonald, J.L.
(1998) Properties of phonological markers that affect the acquisition of gender-like subclasses. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(2), 218–245. Crossref link
Gathercole, S.E., Pickering, S.J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H.
(2004) The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 177–190. Crossref link
Gil, D.
(2009) How much grammar does it take to sail a boat? In G. Sampson, D. Gil, & P. Trudgill (Eds.), Language complexity as an evolving variable (pp. 19–33). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gordon, R.G.
(Ed.) (2005) Ethnologue: Languages of the world, Fifteenth Edition. Dallas, TX: SIL International. Retrieved from http://​archive​.ethnologue​.com​/15/
Hart, B., & Risley, T.R.
(1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Hockett, C.F.
(1966) The problem of universals in language. In J.H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of language, 2nd Edition, Volume 2 (pp. 1–29). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hoff, E.
(2003) The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74(5), 1368–1378. Crossref link
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V.A., & Fernald, A.
(2008) Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11(6), F31–F39. Crossref link
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S.
(2002) Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45(3), 337–374. Crossref link
Imaizumi, H., Kunimatsu, S., & Isei, T.
(2000) Sound propagation and speech transmission in a branching underground tunnel. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 108(2), 632–642. Crossref link
Jelinek, E., & Demers, R.A.
(1994) Predicates and pronominal arguments in Straits Salish. Language, 70(4), 697–736. Crossref link
Jespersen, O.
(1922) Language: Its nature, development and origin. London: George Allan & Unwin.
Johnston, J.R., & Wong, M.-Y.
A (2002) Cultural differences in beliefs and practices concerning talk to children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(5), 916–926. Crossref link
Keller, R.
(1994) On language change: The invisible hand in language. (B. Nerlich, Trans.). London: Routledge.
Kinzler, K.D., Shutts, K., DeJesus, J., & Spelke, E.S.
(2009) Accent trumps race in guiding children’s social preferences. Social Cognition, 27(4), 623–634. Crossref link
Kuczaj, S.A.
(1979) Evidence for a language learning strategy: On the relative ease of acquisition of prefixes and suffixes. Child Development, 50(1), 1–13. Crossref link
Lasnik, H.
(2002) The minimalist program in syntax. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(10), 432–437. Crossref link
Levinson, S.C.
(2003) Language and mind: Let’s get the issues straight! In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought (pp. 25–46). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Levinson, S.C., & Wilkins, D.P.
(2006) Grammars of space: Explorations in cognitive diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref link
Lewis, R.L.
(1996) Interference in short-term memory: The magical number two (or three) in sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(1), 93–115. Crossref link
Lupyan, G., & Dale, R.
(2010) Language structure is partly determined by social structure. PLoS ONE, 5(1), e8559. Crossref link
MacWhinney, B.
(1983) Miniature linguistic systems as tests of the use of universal operating principles in second-language learning by children and adults. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 12(5), 467–478. Crossref link
Maddieson, I., Bhattacharya, T., Smith, D.E., & Croft, W.
(2011) Geographical distribution of phonological complexity. Linguistic Typology, 15(2), 267–279. Crossref link
McWhorter, J.H.
(2001) The world’s simplest grammars are creole grammars. Linguistic Typology, 5(2-3), 125–166. Crossref link
(2002) What happened to English? Diachronica, 19(2), 217–272. Crossref link
(2007) Language interrupted: Signs of non-native acquisition in standard language grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Crossref link
Meyer, J.
(2004) Bioacoustics of human whistled languages: An alternative approach to the cognitive processes of language. Anais Da Academia Brasileira de Ciências, 76(2), 406–412. Crossref link
Mithun, M.
(1984) How to avoid subordination. In Proceedings of the Tenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 493–509). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.
Monaghan, P., Christiansen, M.H., & Fitneva, S.A.
(2011) The arbitrariness of the sign: Learning advantages from the structure of the vocabulary. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(3), 325–347. Crossref link
Nettle, D.
(1996) Language diversity in West Africa: An ecological approach. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 15(4), 403–438. Crossref link
(1998a) Coevolution of phonology and the lexicon in twelve languages of West Africa. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 5(3), 240–245. Crossref link
(1998b) Explaining global patterns of language diversity. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 17(4), 354–374. Crossref link
Nowak, M.A., Komarova, N.L., & Niyogi, P.
(2001) Evolution of universal grammar. Science, 291(5501), 114–118. Crossref link
Perkins, R.D.
(1992) Deixis, grammar, and culture. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Crossref link
Piatelli-Palmarini, M., & Uriagereka, J.
(2004) The immune syntax: The evolution of the language virus. In L. Jenkins (Ed.), Variation and Universals in biolinguistics (pp. 341–377). Oxford: Elsevier.
Pinker, S.
(1994) The language instinct. New York, NY: Harper Collins. Crossref link
Pinker, S., & Bloom, P.
(1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13(4), 707–784. Crossref link
Premack, D.
(1986) Gavagai! or the future history of the animal language controversy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Richman, A.L., Miller, P.M., & LeVine, R.A.
(1992) Cultural and educational variations in maternal responsiveness. Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 614–621. Crossref link
Sampson, G., Gil, D., & Trudgill, P.
(2009) Language complexity as an evolving variable. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sapir, E.
(1921) Language: An introduction to the study of speech. New York: Hartcourt, Brace & World.
Slobin, D.I.
(1979) Psycholinguistics, 2nd Edition . Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman & Co.
(1985) Crosslinguistic evidence for the language making capacity. In D.I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Volume 2: Theoretical issues (pp. 1157–1256). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
(1997) The universal, the typological, and the particular in acquisition. In E. Wanner & L.R. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 1–39). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Slobin, D.I., & Bever, T.G.
(1982) Children use canonical sentence schemas: A crosslinguistic study of word order and inflections. Cognition, 12(3), 229–265. Crossref link
St. Clair, M.C., Monaghan, P., & Ramscar, M.
(2009) Relationships between language structure and language learning: The suffixing preference and drammatical categorization. Cognitive Science, 33(7), 1317–1329. Crossref link
Tamis-LeMonda, C.S., Song, L., Leavell, A.S., Kahana-Kalman, R., & Yoshikawa, H.
(2012) Ethnic differences in mother-infant language and gestural communications are associated with specific skills in infants: Mother-infant communications. Developmental Science, 15(3), 384–397. Crossref link
Thompson, L.C.
(1987) A vietnamese reference grammar. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.
Thurston, W.R.
(1989) How exoteric languages build a lexicon: Esoterogeny in West New Britain. In R. Harlow & R. Cooper (Eds.), Papers from the Fifth International Conference on Austronesian Linguistics (pp. 555–579). Auckland: Linguistic Society of New Zealand.
Trudgill, P.
(1988) On the role of dialect contact and interdialect in linguistic change. In J. Fisiak (Ed.), Historical Dialectology. Regional and social (pp. 547–563). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. Crossref link
(1989) Contact and isolation in linguistic change. In L.E. Breivik & E.H. Jahr (Eds.), Language change: Contributions to the study of its causes (pp. 227–238). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. Crossref link
(1993) Dialect typology: Phonological aspects. In G. Aurrekoetxea & X. Videgain (Eds.), Nazioarteko Dialektologia Blitzarra: Agiriak / Actas del congreso internacional de dialectología (pp. 659–666). Bilbao: Euskaltzainda.
(2001a) Contact and simplification: Historical baggage and directionality in linguistic change. Linguistic Typology, 5(2-3), 371–374.
(2001b) Sociolinguistics: An introduction to language and society, Fourth Edition. London: Penguin.
Vogt, P., & Mastin, J.D.
(2013) Rural and urban differences in language socialization and early vocabulary development in Mozambique. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, M. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3787–3792). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Weiner, J.
(1995) The beak of the Finch: A story of evolution in our time. New York, NY: Vintage.
White, D.R.
(2007) Standard cross-cultural sample free distribution site (UC Irvine): Comparative research, world cultural comparison, courses and workshops. Retrieved 31 March 2009 from http://​eclectic​.ss​.uci​.edu​/~drwhite​/courses​/index​.html
Wray, A., & Grace, G.W.
(2007) The consequences of talking to strangers: Evolutionary corollaries of socio-cultural influences on linguistic form. Lingua, 117(3), 543–578. Crossref link