Article published in:
Cognitive Linguistic Studies
Vol. 8:1 (2021) ► pp. 133151
References

References

Bargh, J. A. & Chartrand, T. L.
(1999) The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F.
(1992) The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 893–912. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes, C.
(1996) The automatic evaluation effect: Unconditionally automatic attitude activation with a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 185–210. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bindra, D., & Thompson, W. R.
(1953) An evaluation of defecating and urinating as measures of fearfulness. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 46, 43–45.Google Scholar
Bossert, W. H., & Wilson, E. O.
(1963) The analysis of olfactory communication among animals. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 5, 443–469. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Croft, William
(2003) The role of domains in the interpretation of metaphors and metonymies. In: Dirven, René & Pörings, Ralf (Eds.), Metaphor and Metonymy in Comparison and Contrast (pp. 161–206). Berlin/New York: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
De Groot, Jasper H. B., and Monique A. M. Smeets
(2017) Human fear chemosignaling: Evidence from a meta-Analysis. Chemical Senses, 42(8), 663–673. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dirven, René.
(2003) Metonymy and metaphor: Different mental strategies of conceptualisation In: Dirven, René & Pörings, Ralf (Eds.), Metaphor and Metonymy in Comparison and Contrast (pp. 75–112). Berlin/New York: Mouton De Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, J.
(1949) Über das Vorkommen von Schreckstoffen bei Erdkrötenquappen. Experientia, 5, 236. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Finucane, M. L., Alhakami, A., Slovic, P., & Johnson, S. M.
(2000) The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13, 1–17. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frisch, K. V.
(1941) Über einen Schreckstoff der Fischhaut und seine biologische Bedeutung. Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie, 29, 46–145. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frühling, A.
(1955) Über das Harnspritzen und ähnliche Verhaltensweisen beim Meerschweinchen. Säugetierkunde Mitteilungen, 3, 168–171.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G.
(2007) Gut feelings. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Hassin, R. R., Aarts, H., & Ferguson, M. J.
(2005) Automatic goal inferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41(2), 129–140. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Haslam, S. A., Oakes, P. J., Reynolds, K. J., & Turner, J. C.
(1999) Social identity salience and the emergence of stereotype consensus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(7), 809–818. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Haslam, S. Alexander, Turner, John C., Oakes, Penelope J., Reynolds, Katherine J. and Doosje, Bertjan
(2002) From personal pictures in the head to collective tools in the world: how shared stereotypes allow groups to represent and change social reality. In: Craig McGarty, et al. (Eds.), Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups (pp. 157–185). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Herrero-Ruiz, Javier
(2009) Understanding tropes: at the crossroads between pragmatics and cognition. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
(2018) Exaggerating and mitigating through metonymy: The case of situational and cause for effect/effect for cause metonymies. Language and Communication 62, 51–65. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hessler, Peter
(2001) River town: two years on the Yangtze. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Hrbacek, I.
(1950) On the flight reaction of the tadpoles of the common toad caused by chemical substances. Experientia, 6, 100–101. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kövecses, Zoltán, & Radden, Günter
(1998) Metonymy: Developing a cognitive linguistic view. Cognitive Linguistics, 9(1), 37–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, Tsung-Han, Joanne Y. Yew, Tatyana Y. Fedina, Klaus Dreisewerd, Herman A. Dierick, Scott D. Pletcher
(2012) Aging modulates cuticular hydrocarbons and sexual attractiveness in Drosophila melanogaster . The Journal of Experimental Biology, 215, 814–821. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, George
(1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W.
(1987) Foundations of cognitive grammar. Volume 1. Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford California, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
(1984) Active zones. Berkeley Linguistics Society 10, 172-188. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1993) Reference-point Constructions. Cognitive Linguistics 4(1). 1-38. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Littlemore, Jeannette
2015Metonymy: hidden shortcuts in language, thought and communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lübke, Katrin T., and Bettina M. Pause
(2015) Always follow your nose: the functional significance of social chemosignals in human reproduction and survival. Hormones and Behavior, 9.4, E94314. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lübke, K. T., Busch, A., Hoenen, M., Schaal, B. & Pause, B. M.
(2017) Chemosensory anxiety signals prime defensive behavior in prepubertal girls. Physiology & Behavior, 173, 30–33. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McRae, Mary
(2004) How do I talk to you, my white sister? Center for Gender in Organizations Commentaries, 2, 1–4.Google Scholar
Moran, James K., Daniel R. Dietrich, Thomas Elbert, Bettina M. Pause, Lisa Kübler, Roland Weierstall, and Heather Hoffmann
(2015) The scent of blood: A driver of human Behavior? PLoS ONE 10(9), E0137777. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Müller-Velten, H.
(1966) Über den Angstgeruch bei der Hausmaus. Zeitschrift Vergleichende Physiologie, 52, 401–429. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & Turner, J. C.
(1994) Stereotyping and social reality. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Patin, Alexandra, & Pause, Bettina M.
(2015) Human amygdala activations during nasal chemoreception. Neuropsychologia, 78, 171–194. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pfeiffer, W.
(1960) Über die Verbreitung der Schreckreaktion bei Fischen. Naturwissenschaften, 47(1), 23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1963) Alarm substances. Experientia, 19, 113–123. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prehn-Kristensen, Alexander, Christian Wiesner, Til Ole Bergmann, Stephan Wolff, Olav Jansen, Hubertus Maximilian Mehdorn, Roman Ferstl, Bettina M. Pause, and Jan Lauwereyns
(2009) Induction of empathy by the smell of anxiety. PLoS ONE 4.6, E5987. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza, Francisco J.
(2005) Linguistic interpretation and cognition. In: E. Croitoru, D. Tuchel & M. Praisler (Eds.), Cultural matrix reloaded. Romanian Society for English and American Studies Seventh International Conference (pp. 36–64). Bucarest: Didactica Si Pedagogica.Google Scholar
Schaller, M., & Maass, A.
(1989) Illusory correlation and social categorization: Toward an integration of motivational and cognitive factors in stereotype formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(5), 709–721. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, W. A., R. D. Mathews, and R. M. Parkhurst
(1962) Alarm reaction of the top smelt Atherinops affinis (Ayres). Science 138, 681–682. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Spears, Russell, & Manstead, A. S. R.
(1989) The social context of stereotyping and differentiation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 19(2), 101–121. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sprott, Richard L.
(1969) “Fear communication” via odor in inbred mice. Psychological Report, 25, 263–268. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, Henri
(1969) Cognitive aspects of prejudice. Journal of Biosocial Science Suppl, 1, 173–191. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1981) Human groups and social categories: studies in social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, John R.
(2003) Category extension by metonymy and metaphor. In: Dirven, René & Pörings, Ralf (Eds.), Metaphor and metonymy in comparison and contrast (pp. 323–348). Berlin/New York: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Wisman, Arnaud, and IlanShrira
(2015) The Smell of Death: Evidence That Putrescine Elicits Threat Management Mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1274. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wyatt, Tristram D.
(2003) Pheromones and animal behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zajonc, R. B.
(1980) Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zhou, Wen & Chen, Denise
(2009) Fear-Related chemosignalsmodulate recognition of fear in ambiguous facial expressions. Psychological Science, 20(2), 177. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zhou, Wen, & Chen, Denise
(2011) Entangled chemosensory emotion and identity: Familiarity enhances detection of chemosensorily encoded Emotion. Social Neuroscience, 6(3), 270–276. CrossrefGoogle Scholar