Article published In:
Perception, Culture and Language
Edited by Judit Baranyiné Kóczy and Rita Brdar-Szabó
[Cognitive Linguistic Studies 10:2] 2023
► pp. 313341
References
Aikhenvald, A. Y., & Storch, A.
(Eds.) (2013) Perception and cognition in language and culture. Leiden: Brill. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alm-Arvius, C.
(1993) The English verb see: A study in multiple meaning. Göteborg: Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis.Google Scholar
Baker, C. E.
(1999) Seeing clearly: Frame semantic, psycholinguistics, and cross-linguistic approaches to the semantics of the English verb see. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation. Berkeley: University of California.
Baranyiné Kóczy, J.
(2020) Keeping an eye on body parts: Cultural conceptualizations of the ‘eye’ in Hungarian. In I. Kraska-Szlenk (Ed.), Body part terms in conceptualization and language usage (pp. 215–245). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021) The moral eye: A study of Hungarian szem . In M. Baş & I. Kraska-Szlenk (Eds.), Embodiment in cross-linguistic studies: The ‘eye’ (pp. 45–69). Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
Bárczi, G. & Országh, L.
(Eds.) (1978) A magyar nyelv értelmező szótára III. Harmadik kiadás [Dictionary of the Hungarian Language Vol. III. Third edition]. Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó.Google Scholar
Bárczi, G., & Országh, L.
(Eds.) (1979) A magyar nyelv értelmező szótára IV. Harmadik kiadás [Dictionary of the Hungarian language Vol. IV. third edition]. Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó.Google Scholar
(Eds.) (1980a) A magyar nyelv értelmező szótára V. Harmadik kiadás [Dictionary of the Hungarian language Vol. V. third edition]. Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó.Google Scholar
(Eds.) (1980b) A magyar nyelv értelmező szótára VI. Harmadik kiadás [Dictionary of the Hungarian language Vol. VI. third edition]. Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó.Google Scholar
Baş, M., & Kraska-Szlenk, I.
(Eds.) (2021) Embodiment in cross-linguistic studies: The ‘eye’. Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
Brenzinger, M., & Kraska-Szlenk, I.
(Eds.) (2014) The body in language: Comparative studies of linguistic embodiment. Leiden: Brill. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caruso, E. M., & Gino, F.
(2011) Blind ethics: Closing one’s eyes polarizes moral judgments and discourages dishonest behavior. Cognition, 118 (2), 280–285. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ciprianová, E., & Kováčová, Z.
(2018) Figurative ‘eye’ expressions in the conceptualization of emotions and personality traits in Slovak. Jezikoslovlje, 19 (1), 5–38.Google Scholar
Classen, C.
(1993) Worlds of sense: Exploring the senses in history and across cultures. London: Longman.Google Scholar
(Ed.) (2005) The book of touch (1st ed.). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Curry, O. S., Mullins, D. A., & Whitehouse, H.
(2019) Is it good to cooperate? Testing the theory of morality-as-cooperation in 60 societies. Current Anthropology, 60 (1), 47–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Danesi, M.
(1990) Thinking is seeing: Visual metaphors and the nature of abstract thought. Semiotica, 80 (3–4), 221–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Vries, L.
(2013) Seeing, hearing and thinking in Korowai, a language of West Papua. In A. Y. Aikhenvald & A. Storch (Eds.), Perception and cognition in language and culture (pp. 111–136). Leiden: Brill. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Divjak, D.
(2015) Exploring the grammar of perception: A case study using data from Russian. Functions of Language, 22 (1), 44–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, N., & Wilkins, D.
(2000) In the mind’s ear: The semantic extensions of perception verbs in Australian languages. Language, 76 (3), 546–592. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gisborne, N.
(2010) The event structure of perception verbs. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Handfield, T., & Thrasher, J.
(2019) Two of a kind: Are norms of honor a species of morality?. Biology & Philosophy, 34 1, Article 39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holland, D., & Quinn, N.
(Eds.) 1987Cultural models in language and thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Howes, D.
(Ed.) (1991) The varieties of sensory experience: A sourcebook in the anthropology of the senses. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I.
(1999) Metaphorical mappings in the sense of smell. In R. W. Gibbs, Jr. & G. J. Steen (Eds.), Metaphor in cognitive linguistics (pp. 29–45). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) MIND-AS-BODY as a cross-linguistic conceptual metaphor. Miscelánea: A Journal of English and American Studies, 25 1, 93–119.Google Scholar
(2008) Vision metaphors for the intellect: Are they really cross-linguistic?. Atlantis, 30 (1), 15–33.Google Scholar
Johnson, M.
(1987) The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1993) Moral imagination: Implications of cognitive science for ethics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M., & Lenci, A.
(2011) Verbs of visual perception in Italian FrameNet. Constructions and Frames, 3 (1), 9–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kerr, J.
(2002) The open door: Hospitality and honour in twelfth/early thirteenth-century England. History, 87 (287), 322–335. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) ‘Welcome the coming and speed the parting guest’: Hospitality in twelfth-century England. Journal of Medieval History, 33 (2), 130–146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kövecses, Z.
(1999) Metaphor: Does it constitute or reflect cultural models?. In R. W. Gibbs, Jr. & G. J. Steen (Eds.), Metaphor in cognitive linguistics (pp. 167–188). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Where metaphors come from: Reconsidering context in metaphor. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kraska-Szlenk, I.
(2014) Semantics of body part terms: General trends and a case study of Swahili. München: Lincom.Google Scholar
Lajos, K.
(2021) “Nem tudnak újjal rámutatni”: A test a szexualitásról való beszéd csíkszentdomonkosi frazeológiájában [“No-one can point their fingers at her”: The body in the phraseology of talking about sexuality in Csikszentdomonkos]. In K. Lajos & J. Pieldner (Eds.), Zsuzsa könyve. Tanulmányok Tapodi Zsuzsa születésnapjára (pp. 217–227). Kolozsvár: Erdélyi Múzeum Egyesület.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W.
(1991) Concept, image, and symbol: The cognitive basis of grammar. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Maalej, Z.
(2011) Figurative dimensions of 3ayn ‘eye’ in Tunisian Arabic. In Z. Maalej & N. Yu (Eds.), Embodiment via body parts: Studies from various languages and cultures (pp. 213–240). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maalej, Z., & Yu, N.
Majid, A., Roberts, S. G., Cilissen, L., Emmorey, K., Nicodemus, B., O’Grady, L., Woll, B., LeLan, B., de Sousa, H., Cansler, B. L., Shayan, S., de Vos, C., Senft, G., Enfield, N. J., Razak, R. A., Fedden, S., Tufvesson, S., Dingemanse, M., Ozturk, O.,… Levinson, S. C.
(2018) Differential coding of perception in the world’s languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115 (45), 11369–11376. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Occhi, D. J.
(2011) A cultural-linguistic look at Japanese ‘eye’ expressions. In Z. Maalej & N. Yu (Eds.), Embodiment via body parts: Studies from various languages and cultures (pp. 171–194). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Proos, M.
(2019) Polysemy of the Estonian perception verb nägema ‘to see’. In L. J. Speed, C. O’Meara, L. San Roque & A. Majid (Eds.), Perception metaphors (pp. 231–252). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) Feeling your neighbour: An experimental approach to the polysemy of tundma ‘to feel’ in Estonian. Language and Cognition, 12 (2), 282–309. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
San Roque, L., Kendrick, K. H., Norcliffe, E., Brown, P., Defina, R., Dingemanse, M., Dirksmeyer, T., Enfield, N. J., Floyd, S., Hammond, J., Rossi, J., Tufvesson, S., van Putten, S., & Majid, A.
(2015) Vision verbs dominate in conversation across cultures, but the ranking of non-visual verbs varies. Cognitive linguistics, 26 (1), 31–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
San Roque, L., Kendrick, K. H., Norcliffe, E., & Majid, A.
(2018) Universal meaning extensions of perception verbs are grounded in interaction. Cognitive Linguistics, 29 (3), 371–406. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sekuler, R., & Blake, R.
(1994) Perception. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Sharifian, F.
(2011a) Conceptualizations of cheshm ‘eye’ in Persian. In Z. Maalej & N. Yu (Eds.), Embodiment via body parts: Studies from various languages and cultures (pp. 197–212). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sharifian, F., Dirven, R., Yu, N., & Niemeier, S.
(Eds.) (2008) Culture, body, and language: Conceptualizations of internal body organs across cultures and languages. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shehu, A.
(2020) The conceptualisation of ido ‘eye’ in Hausa. In I. Kraska−Szlenk (Ed.), Body part terms in conceptualization and language usage (pp. 247–268). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sweetser, E.
(1990) From etymology to pragmatics: Metaphorical and cultural aspects of semantic structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Szily, K.
(1902) A magyar nyelvújítás szótára: A kedveltebb képzők és képzésmódok jegyzékével [Dictionary of the Hungarian language reform: With a list of the most popular derivative suffixes and derivation modes]. Budapest: Hornyánszky.Google Scholar
Usoniene, A.
(2001) On direct/indirect perception with verbs of seeing and seeming in English and Lithuanian. Working papers/Lund University, Department of Linguistics and Phonetics, 48 1, 163–182.Google Scholar
Vainik, E.
(2018) Emotion meets taste: Taste-motivated emotion terms in Estonian. Folklore: Electronic Journal of Folklore, 71 1, 129–154.Google Scholar
Vanhove, M.
(2008) Semantic associations between sensory modalities, prehension and mental perceptions: A crosslinguistic perspective. In M. Vanhove (Ed.), From polysemy to semantic change: Towards a typology of lexical semantic associations (pp. 341–370). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Viberg, Å.
(1983) The verbs of perception: A Typological Study. Linguistics, 21 (1), 123–162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Swedish verbs of perception from a typological and contrastive perspective. In M. Á. Gómez González, J. L. Mackenzie & E. M. González Álvarez (Eds.), Languages and cultures in contrast and comparison (pp. 123–172). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whitt, R. J.
(2010) Evidentiality, polysemy, and the verbs of perception in English and German. In G. Diewald & E. Smirnova (Eds.), Linguistic realization of evidentiality in European languages (pp. 249–278). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winter, B., Perlman, M., & Majid, A.
(2018) Vision dominates in perceptual language: English sensory vocabulary is optimized for usage. Cognition, 179 1, 213–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yu, N.
(2001) What does our face mean to us?. Pragmatics & Cognition, 9 (1), 1–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004) The eyes for sight and mind. Journal of Pragmatics, 36 (4), 663–686. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Metaphor from body and culture. In R. W. Gibbs, Jr (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 247–261). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Metaphorical character of moral cognition: A comparative and decompositional analysis. Metaphor and Symbol, 30 (3), 163–183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Spatial metaphors for morality: A perspective from Chinese. Metaphor and Symbol, 31 (2), 108–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2022) The moral metaphor system: A conceptual metaphor approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar