Article published in:
Dutch Journal of Applied Linguistics
Vol. 6:1 (2017) ► pp. 119146

Full-text

Syllable type development in toddlers acquiring Dutch
References

References

Carson, P., Klee, T., Carson, D. K., & Hime, L. K.
(2003) Phonological profiles of 2-year-olds with delayed language development: Predicting clinical outcomes at age 3. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 28–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Demuth, K.
(1997) Variation in acquisition: An optimal approach. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistic Club.Google Scholar
Efron, B.
(1979) Bootstrap methods: Another look at the jackknife. The Annals of Statistics, 7, 1–26. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Elffers, B., Van Bael, C., & Strik, H.
(2005) ADAPT: Algorithm for dynamic alignment of phonetic transcriptions. (Internal report retrieved from the Department of Language and Speech). Radboud University, Nijmegen. Available from http://​lands​.let​.ru​.nl​/literature​/elffers​.2005​.1​.pdf
Fikkert, P.
(1994) On the acquisition of prosodic stress. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Leiden.Google Scholar
Goldstein, B., & Cintron, P.
(2001) An investigation of phonological skills in Puerto Rican Spanish-speaking 2-year-olds. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 15(5), 343–361. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grijzenhout, J., & Joppen-Hellwig, S.
(2002) The lack of onsets in German child phonology. In I. Lasser (Ed.), The process of language acquistion (pp. 319–339). Frankfurt: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Hesterberg, T. C., Moore, D. S., Monaghan, S., Clipson, A., & Epstein, R.
(2005) Bootstrap methods and permutation tests. New York, NY: W. H. Freeman.[ p. 145 ]Google Scholar
Hutchins, T. L., Brannick, M., Bryant, J. B., & Silliman, E. R.
(2005) Methods for controlling amount of talk: Difficulties, considerations and recommendations. First Language, 25(3), 347–363. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Keren-Portnoy, T., Majorano, M., & Vihman, M. M.
(2009) From phonetics to phonology: The emergence of first words in Italian. Journal of Child Language, 36, 235–267. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, C. C., Schiller, N. O., & Levelt, W. J. M.
(2000) The acquisition of syllable types. Language Acquisition, 8(3), 237–264. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(2000) The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Molemans, I., Van Severen, L., Van den Berg, R., Govaerts, P., & Gillis, S.
(2010) Spraakzaamheid van Nederlandstalige baby’s en peuters [Speech volubility in Dutch-speaking babies and toddlers]. Logopedie, 23, 12–23.Google Scholar
Paul, R., & Jennings, P.
(1992) Phonological behavior in toddlers with slow expressive language development. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 99–108. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pharr, A. B., Bernstein Ratner, N., & Rescorla, L.
(2000) Syllable structure development of toddlers with expressive specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 429–449. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L., & Bernstein Ratner, N.
(1996) Phonetic profiles of toddlers with specific expressive language impairment (SLI-E). Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39(1), 153–166. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Fletcher, S. L., & Freundenthal, D.
(2008) How big is big enough? In H. Behrens (Ed.), Corpora in language acquisition research: History, methods, perspectives (pp. 1–24). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schiller, N. O., Meyer, A. S., Baayen, H. R., & Levelt, W. J. M.
(1996) A comparison of lexeme and speech syllables in Dutch. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 3(1), 8–28. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C., & Cooper, J. A.
(1984) Patterns of early lexical and phonological development. Journal of Child Language, 1(1), 247–271.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C.
(1987) Phonological skills of 2-year-olds. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 18, 323–329. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. Journal of Child Language, 38(1), 1–34. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Taelman, H., Durieux, G., & Gillis, S.
(2005) Notes on Ingram’s whole-word measures for phonological development. Journal of Child Language, 32, 391–405. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thal, D. J., Oroz, M., & McCaw, V.
(1995) Phonological and lexical development in normal and late-talking toddlers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 407–424.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Stahl, D.
(2004) Sampling children’s spontaneous speech: How much is enough? Journal of Child Language, 31, 101–121. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van den Berg, R.
(2012) Syllables inside out. A longitudinal study of the development of syllable types in toddlers acquiring Dutch: A comparison between hearing impaired children with a cochlear implant and normally hearing children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Antwerp.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Ferguson, C. A., & Elbert, M.
(1986) Phonological development from babbling to speech: Common tendencies and individual differences. Applied Psycholinguistics, 7, 3–40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M., & McCune, L.
(1994) When is a word a word? Journal of Child Language, 21, 517–542. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M., DePaolis, R. A., & Keren-Portnoy, T.
(2009) A dynamic systems approach to babbling and words. In E. L. Bavin (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language (pp. 163–182). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.[ p. 146 ] CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vogel Sosa, A., & Stoel-Gammon, C.
(2006) Patterns of intra-word phonological variability during the second year of life. Journal of Child Language, 33, 31–50. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Watson, M. M., & Scukanec, G. P.
(1997) Profiling the phonological abilities of 2-year-olds: A longitudinal investigation. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 13, 3–14. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zink, I., & Lejaegere, M.
(2002) N-CDI’s Lijsten voor communicatieve ontwikkeling [Dutch version of MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories]. Leuven: ACCO.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Haaften, Leenke, Sanne Diepeveen, Lenie den Engel‐Hoek, Bert Swart & Ben Maassen
2020. Speech sound development in typically developing 2–7‐year‐old Dutch‐speaking children: A normative cross‐sectional study. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 55:6  pp. 971 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 29 september 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.