References
Akmajian, A., Farmer, A. K., Bickmore, L., Demers, R. A., & Harnish, R. M.
(2017) Linguistics: An Introduction to Language and Communication (seventh edition). Cambridge, Mass.: The Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, S. R.
(2006) Doctor Dolittle′s Delusion – Animals and the Uniqueness of Human Language. New Haven, Conn.; London: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Andrews, K.
(2016) Animal Cognition. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2016) Retrieved from [URL]
Arbib, M. A.
(2012) How the brain got language: The mirror systems hypothesis. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ariel, M.
(2015) Doubling up: Two upper bounds for scalars. Linguistics, 53(3), 561–610. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arnold, K., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2006) Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature, 441(7091), 303–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Call combinations in monkeys: Compositional or idiomatic expressions? Brain and Language, 120(3), 303–309. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aubin, T., & Jouventin, P.
(2002) Localisation of an acoustic signal in a noisy environment: the display call of the king penguin Aptenodytes patagonicus. Journal of Experimental Biology 205(24), 3793–3798.Google Scholar
Balari, S., & Lorenzo, G.
(2016) Evo-devo of Language and Cognition. Evolutionary Developmental Biology, 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bar-On, D.
(2018) Communicative intentions, expressive communication, and origins of meaning. In K. Andrews & Beck, Jacob (Eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of Animal Minds (pp. 301–312). London; New York: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Bar-On, D., & Moore, R.
(2018) Pragmatic interpretation and signaler-receiver asymmetries in animal communication. In K. Andrews & J. Beck (Eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of Animal Minds (pp. 291–300). London; New York: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Beckner, C., Ellis, N. C., Blythe, R., Holland, J., Bybee, J., Ke, J., Christiansen, M. H., Larsen-Freeman, D., Croft, W., Schoenemann, T., & Five Graces Group
(2009) Language is a Complex Adaptive System: Position Paper. Language Learning, 59 Suppl. 11, 1–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Benítez-Burraco, A., & Boeckx, C.
(2014) Universal Grammar and Biological Variation: An EvoDevo Agenda for Comparative Biolinguistics. Biological Theory, 9(2), 122–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berthet, M., Mesbahi, G., Pajot, A., Cäsar, C., Neumann, C., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2019) Titi monkeys combine alarm calls to create probabilistic meaning. Science Advances, 5(5), eaav3991. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D.
(2009) Adam’s Tongue: How Humans Made Language, How Language Made Humans. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Blumenthal-Dramé, A.
(2012) Entrenchment in Usage-Based Theories: What Corpus Data Do and Do Not Reveal About The Mind (1 edition). Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Börjars, K., Vincent, N. & Walkden, G.
(2015) On Constructing a Theory of Grammatical Change. Transactions of the Philological Society 113(3). 363–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brône, G., & Zima, E.
(2014) Towards a dialogic construction grammar: Ad hoc routines and resonance activation. Cognitive Linguistics, 25(3), 457–495. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. L.
(2010) Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W., Cartmill, E., Genty, E., Graham, K. E., Hobaiter, C., & Tanner, J.
(2017) Great ape gestures: Intentional communication with a rich set of innate signals. Animal Cognition 20(4), 755–769. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cartmill, E. A., & Hobaiter, C.
(2019) Developmental perspectives on primate gesture: 100 years in the making. Animal Cognition, 22(4), 53–459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cartmill, E. A., & Maestripieri, D.
(2012) Socio-Cognitive Specializations in Nonhuman Primates: Evidence from Gestural Communication. The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Evolutionary Psychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1959) A review of BF Skinner’s Verbal behavior. Language, 35(1), 26–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cienki, A.
(2017) Utterance Construction Grammar (UCxG) and the variable multimodality of constructions. Linguistics Vanguard, 3(s1). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E.
(2009) Words as constructions. In V. Evans & S. Pourcel (Eds.), New Directions in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 201–223). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dancygier, B., & Vandelanotte, L.
(2017) Internet memes as multimodal constructions. Cognitive Linguistics, 28(3), 565–598. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Boer, B., Sandler, W., & Kirby, S.
(2012) New perspectives on duality of patterning: Introduction to the special issue. Language and Cognition, 4(4), 251–259. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deacon, T. W.
(1997) The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S.
(2020) How We Learn: Why Brains Learn Better Than Any Machine… for Now. London: Viking.Google Scholar
Diessel, H.
(2019) The Grammar Network: How Linguistic Structure Is Shaped by Language Use. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M.
(2018) Redrawing the margins of language: Lessons from research on ideophones. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 3(1), 4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Divjak, D.
(2019) Frequency in Language: Memory, Attention and Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S.
(2017) Why are there so many explanations for primate brain evolution? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1727) 20160244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eason, P., & Vanderhoff, E. N.
(2009) The response of American robins (Turdus migratorius) to aerial alarms. Behaviour, 146(3), 415–427. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Endress, A. D., Cahill, D., Block, S., Watumull, J., & Hauser, M. D.
(2009) Evidence of an Evolutionary Precursor to Human Language Affixation in a Non-Human Primate. Biology Letters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engesser, S., & Townsend, S. W.
(2019) Combinatoriality in the vocal systems of nonhuman animals. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, e1493.Google Scholar
Evans, V.
(2019) Cognitive Linguistics. A complete guide. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Fasold, R. W., & Connor-Linton, J.
(Eds.) (2014) An Introduction to Language and Linguistics (2nd edition). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E.
(1994) Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5), 1–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fischer, J.
(2002) Developmental modifications in the vocal behavior of non-human primates. In A. A. Ghazanfar (Ed.), Primate audition: Ethology and neurobiology (pp. 109–125). Boca Raton: CRC Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Primate vocal production and the riddle of language evolution. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(1), 72–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fischer, J., Wheeler, B. C., & Higham, J. P.
(2015) Is there any evidence for vocal learning in chimpanzee food calls? Current Biology, 25(21), R1028–R1029. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. T.
(2017) Empirical approaches to the study of language evolution. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(1), 3–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) Animal cognition and the evolution of human language: Why we cannot focus solely on communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1789) 20190046. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Floyd, S., & Goldberg, A. E.
(2020) Children make use of relationships across meanings in word learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frank, R. M., & Gontier, N.
(2010) On Constructing a Research Model for Historical Cognitive Linguistics (HCL): Some Theoretical Considerations. In M. E. Winters, H. Tissari, & K. Allan (Eds.), Historical Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 31–69). Berlin; New York: de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genty, E., Breuer, T., Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W.
(2009) Gestural communication of the gorilla (Gorilla gorilla): Repertoire, intentionality and possible origins. Animal Cognition, 12(3), 527–546. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genty, E., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2015) Iconic gesturing in bonobos. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 8(1), e992742. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, K. R.
(2012) Language or protolanguage? A review of the ape language literature. In M. Tallerman & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution (pp. 46–58). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gill, S. A., & Bierema, A. M.-K.
(2013) On the Meaning of Alarm Calls: A Review of Functional Reference in Avian Alarm Calling. Ethology, 119(6), 449–461. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E.
(1995) Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago, London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(2013) Constructionist Approaches. In T. Hoffmann & G. Trousdale (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar (pp. 15–31). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gong, T., Shuai, L., & Wu, Y.
(2018) Rethinking foundations of language from a multidisciplinary perspective. Physics of Life Reviews, 26–271, 120–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
González-Forero, M., & Gardner, A.
(2018) Inference of ecological and social drivers of human brain-size evolution. Nature, 557(7706), 554–557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Håkansson, G., & Westander, J.
(2013) Communication in Humans and Other Animals. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Halina, M., Rossano, F., & Tomasello, M.
(2013) The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures. Animal Cognition, 16(4), 653–666. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hall, J. A., Horgan, T. G., & Murphy, N. A.
(2019) Nonverbal Communication. Annual Review of Psychology, 70(1), 271–294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hammerschmidt, K., & Fischer, J.
(2008) Constraints in primate vocal production. In K. Oller & U. Griebel (Eds.), The Evolution of communicative creativity: From fixed signals to contextual flexibility (pp. 93–119). Cambridge, Mass.: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartmann, S.
(2020) Language change and language evolution: Cousins, siblings, twins? Glottotheory. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D.
(1996) The Evolution of Communication by Marc D. Hauser. Cambridge, Mass.: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.Google Scholar
Hebets, E. A., Barron, A. B., Balakrishnan, C. N., Hauber, M. E., Mason, P. H., & Hoke, K. L.
(2016) A systems approach to animal communication. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 283(1826) 20152889. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heine, B. & Kuteva, T.
(2007) The Genesis of Grammar: A Reconstruction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heine, B., & Kuteva, T.
(2012) Grammaticalization Theory as a Tool for Reconstructing Language Evolution. In M. Tallerman & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution (pp. 511–527). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W.
(2011) The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Animal Cognition, 14(5), 745–767. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) The meanings of chimpanzee gestures. Current Biology, 24(14), 1596–1600. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hockett, C. F.
(1960) The origin of speech. Scientific American 2031, 89–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E.
(2013) Language Development (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Hopper, P. J., & Traugott, E. C.
(2003) Grammaticalization (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hurford, J. R.
(2012) The Origins of Grammar: Language in the Light of Evolution, Vol. 2. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2007) The Origins of Meaning: Language in the Light of Evolution, Vol. 1. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Imo, W., & Ziegler, E.
(2019) Situierte Konstruktionen. Das Indefinitpronomen man im Kontext der Aushandlung von Einstellungen zu migrationsbedingter Mehrsprachigkeit. In J. Erfurt & S. De Knop (Eds.), Konstruktionsgrammatik und Mehrsprachigkeit (pp. 75–104). Duisburg: Universitätsverlag Rhein-Ruhr.Google Scholar
Inkelas, S.
(2014) Non-concatenative derivation. In R. Lieber & P. Stekauer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of derivational morphology (pp. 169–189). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R.
(2002) Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R., & Wittenberg, E.
(2017) Linear grammar as a possible stepping-stone in the evolution of language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(1), 219–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Janik, V. M., & Slater, P. J.
(2000) The different roles of social learning in vocal communication. Animal Behaviour, 60(1), 1–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johansson, S.
(2016) Protolanguage Possibilities in a Construction Grammar Framework. In S. G. Roberts, C. Cuskley, L. McCrohon, L. Barceló-Coblijn, O. Fehér, & T. Verhoef (Eds.), The Evolution of Language. Proceedings of the 11th International Conference. Retrieved from [URL]
Johnson, E. K., & White, K. S.
(2019) Six Questions in Infant Speech and Language Development. In P. Hagoort (Ed.), Human Language: From Genes and Brains to Behavior (pp. 99–112). Cambridge, Mass. & London, UK: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press..Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Fischer, J.
(2004) Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for ‘fast mapping’. Science (New York, N.Y.), 304(5677), 1682–1683. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Karuza, E. A., Thompson-Schill, S. L., & Bassett, D. S.
(2016) Local patterns to global architectures: influences of network topology on human learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20(8), 629–640. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kay, P., & Fillmore, C. J.
(1999) Grammatical constructions and linguistic generalizations: The What’s X doing Y? construction. Language, 75(1), 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keller, R.
(1994) Sprachwandel: Von der unsichtbaren Hand in der Sprache. Tübingen, Basel: Francke.Google Scholar
Kemmer, S., & Barlow, M.
(2000) Introduction: A usage-based conception of language. In M. Barlow & S. Kemmer (Eds.), Usage-Based Models of Language. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Kershenbaum, A., Blumstein, D. T., Roch, M. A., Akçay, Ç., Backus, G., Bee, M. A., … Zamora-Gutierrez, V.
(2016) Acoustic sequences in non-human animals: A tutorial review and prospectus. Biological Reviews, 91(1), 13–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kersken, V., Gómez, J.-C., Liszkowski, U., Soldati, A., & Hobaiter, C.
(2019) A gestural repertoire of 1-to 2-year-old human children: In search of the ape gestures. Animal Cognition, 22(4), 577–595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kirby, S., Tamariz, M., Cornish, H. & Smith, K.
(2015) Compression and communication in the cultural evolution of linguistic structure. Cognition 1411, 87–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kiriazis, J., & Slobodchikoff, C. N.
(2006) Perceptual specificity in the alarm calls of Gunnison’s prairie dogs. Behavioural Processes, 73(1), 29–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kitzmann, C. D., & Caine, N. G.
(2009) Marmoset (Callithrix geoffroyi) Food-Associated Calls are Functionally Referential. Ethology, 115(5), 439–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. W.
(1987) Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Vol. 1: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
(1988) A Usage-Based Model. In B. Rudzka-Ostyn (Ed.), Topics in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 127–161). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Larsen, S. E.
(1997) Ferdinand de Saussure und seine Nachfolger. In R. Posner, K. Robering, & T. A. Sebeok (Eds.), Semiotik. Ein Handbuch zu den zeichentheoretischen Grundlagen von Natur und Kultur (pp. 2040–2073). Berlin; New York: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Lepic, R.
(2019) A usage-based alternative to “lexicalization” in sign language linguistics. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 4(1), 23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K., & Oña, L.
(2018) Different Approaches to Meaning in Primate Gestural and Vocal Communication. Frontiers in Psychology, 91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K., Waller, B. M., Slocombe, K. E., & Burrows, A. M.
(2014) Primate communication: A multimodal approach. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lyn, H.
(2012) Apes and the Evolution of Language: Taking Stock of 40 Years of Research. In T. K. Shackelford & J. Vonk (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Evolutionary Psychology (pp. 356–378). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Macedonia, J. M., & Evans, C. S.
(1993) Essay on Contemporary Issues in Ethology: Variation among Mammalian Alarm Call Systems and the Problem of Meaning in Animal Signals. Ethology, 93(3), 177–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(2015) Introduction: Language emergence. In B. MacWhinney & W. O’Grady (Eds.), Handbook of language emergence (pp. 1–32). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian
(2015) Emergentism. In E. Dąbrowska & D. Divjak (Eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 689–706). Berlin; New York: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Manrique, H. M., & Walker, M. J.
(2017) Early Evolution of Human Memory. London: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Manser, M. B.
(2001) The acoustic structure of suricates’ alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 268(1483), 2315–2324. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martins, P. T., & Boeckx, C.
(2019) Language evolution and complexity considerations: The no half-Merge fallacy. PLOS Biology, 17(11), e3000389. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Massip-Bonet, À., & Bastardas-Boada, A.
(Eds.) (2013) Complexity Perspectives on Language, Communication and Society. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miyagawa, S., & Clarke, E.
(2019) Systems Underlying Human and Old World Monkey Communication: One, Two, or Infinite. Frontiers in Psychology, 101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Molesti, S., Meguerditchian, A., & Bourjade, M.
(2019) Gestural communication in olive baboons (Papio anubis): Repertoire and intentionality. Animal Cognition, 1–22.Google Scholar
Mollica, F., & Piantadosi, S. T.
(2019) Humans store about 1.5 megabytes of information during language acquisition. Royal Society Open Science, 6(3), 181393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moore, R.
(2014) Ape gestures: interpreting chimpanzee and bonobo minds. Current Biology, 24(14), R645–R647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Müller, C.
(2013) Introduction. In C. Müller, A. J. Cienki, E. Fricke, S. H. Ladewig, D. McNeill, & S. Tessendorf (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (pp. 1–6). Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Nölle, J., Staib, M., Fusaroli, R. & Tylén, K.
(2018) The emergence of systematicity: How environmental and communicative factors shape a novel communication system. Cognition 1811. 93–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oña, L. S., Sandler, W., & Liebal, K.
(2019) A stepping stone to compositionality in chimpanzee communication. PeerJ, 71, e7623. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ouattara, K., Lemasson, A., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2009) Campbell’s Monkeys Use Affixation to Alter Call Meaning. PLOS ONE, 4(11), e7808. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pleyer, M.
(2017) Protolanguage and Mechanisms of Meaning Construal in Interaction. Language Sciences, 631, 69–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pleyer, M., & Hartmann, S.
(2019) Constructing a consensus on language evolution? Convergences and differences between biolinguistic and usage-based approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 101, 2537. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pleyer, M., & Lindner, N.
(2014) Constructions, Construal and Cooperation in the Evolution of Language. In E. A. Cartmill, S. Roberts, H. Lyn, & H. Cornish (Eds.), The Evolution of Language. Proceedings of the 10th Conference (pp. 244–251). World Scientific. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pleyer, M., & Winters, J.
(2014) Integrating Cognitive Linguistics and Language Evolution Research. Theoria et Historia Scientiarium, 111, 19–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M.
(2012) Symbolic communication in the grey Parrot. In T. K. Shackelford & J. Vonk (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Evolutionary Psychology (pp. 297–319). Retrieved from [URL], [URL]
Petkov, C. I., & ten Cate, C.
(2020) Structured sequence learning: animal abilities, cognitive operations, and language evolution. Topics in Cognitive Science, 12(3), 828–842. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piantadosi, S. T., & Fedorenko, E.
(2017) Infinitely productive language can arise from chance under communicative pressure. Journal of Language Evolution, 2(2), 141–147. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pilley, J. W., & Reid, A. K.
(2011) Border collie comprehends object names as verbal referents. Behavioural Processes, 86(2), 184–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pollick, A. S., & De Waal, F. B.
(2007) Ape gestures and language evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(19), 8184–8189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Price, T., Wadewitz, P., Cheney, D., Seyfarth, R., Hammerschmidt, K., & Fischer, J.
(2015) Vervets revisited: A quantitative analysis of alarm call structure and context specificity. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 1–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Progovac, L.
(2015) Evolutionary syntax (First edition). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019a) A Critical Introduction to Language Evolution Current Controversies and Future Prospects. Cham: Switzerland. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019b) Minimalism in the Light of Biology: What to Retain and What to Discard? Frontiers in Psychology, 101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rizzi, L.
(2016) Monkey morpho-syntax and merge-based systems. Theoretical Linguistics, 42(1–2), 139–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, A. I., Vick, S.-J., Roberts, S. G. B., Buchanan-Smith, H. M., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2012) A structure-based repertoire of manual gestures in wild chimpanzees: Statistical analyses of a graded communication system. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(5), 578–589. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C.
(2014) Understanding Child Language Acquisition (1 edition). London; New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Murphy, J., Sevcik, R. A., Brakke, K. E., Williams, S. L., & Rumbaugh, D. M.
(1993) Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(3–4), 1–222.Google Scholar
Saxton, M.
(2017) Child Language: Acquisition and Development (Second edition). Los Angeles London New Delhi Singapore Washington DC Melbourne: SAGE Publications Ltd.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A., & Clay, Z.
(2015) Contextually variable signals can be functionally referential. Animal Behaviour, Complete (100), e1–e8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schel, A. M., Machanda, Z., Townsend, S. W., Zuberbühler, K., & Slocombe, K. E.
(2013) Chimpanzee food calls are directed at specific individuals. Animal Behaviour, 86(5), 955–965. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schlenker, P., Chemla, E., Arnold, K., Lemasson, A., Ouattara, K., Keenan, S., … Zuberbühler, K.
(2014) Monkey semantics: Two ‘dialects’ of Campbell’s monkey alarm calls. Linguistics and Philosophy, 37(6), 439–501. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schlenker, P., Chemla, E., Schel, A. M., Fuller, J., Gautier, J.-P., Kuhn, J., … Zuberbühler, K.
(2016) Formal monkey linguistics. Theoretical Linguistics, 42(1–2), 1–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schlenker, P., Chemla, E., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2017) Semantics and Pragmatics of Monkey Communication. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmid, H.-J.
(2018) Entrenchment and the Psychology of Language Learning: How We Reorganize and Adapt Linguistic Knowledge (1 edition). Berlin, Boston: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Schoenemann, P. T.
(2009) Evolution of brain and language. Language Learning, 591, 162–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) A Complex-Adaptive-Systems Approach to the Evolution of Language and the Brain. In S. Mufwene, C. Coupe, & F. Pellegrino (Eds.), Complexity in Language (pp. 67–100). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwaiger, T.
(2015) Reduplication. In P. O. Müller, I. Ohnheiser, S. Olsen, & F. Rainer (Eds.), Volume 1 Word-Formation, An International Handbook of the Languages of Europe (pp. 467–484). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T.
(2015) Speaking Our Minds: Why Human Communication is Different, and How Language Evolved to Make It Special. London: Pelgrave. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R., Cheney, D. L., & Marler, P.
(1980) Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: Evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science, 210(4471), 801–803. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L.
(2017) The origin of meaning in animal signals. Animal Behaviour, 1241, 339–346. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, A. D. M.
(2008) Protolanguage reconstructed. Interaction Studies 9(1). 100–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, J. D., Zakrzewski, A. C., Johnson, J. M., Valleau, J. C., & Church, B. A.
(2016) Categorization: The View from Animal Cognition. Behavioral Sciences, 6(2), 12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, K.
(2004) The evolution of vocabulary. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 228(1), 127–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, K., Tamariz, M., & Kirby, S.
(2013) Linguistic structure is an evolutionary trade-off between simplicity and expressivity. Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 1348–1353. Retrieved from [URL]
Spranger, M.
(2016) The evolution of grounded spatial language. Berlin: Language Science Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Steels, L.
(2004) Constructivist development of grounded construction grammars. Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Meeting on Association for Computational Linguistics – ACL ’04. Presented at the the 42nd Annual Meeting, Barcelona, Spain. DOI logo
(2011) Introducing Fluid Construction Grammar. In L. Steels (Ed.), Design patterns in fluid construction grammar (pp. 3–30). Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Basics of Fluid Construction Grammar. Constructions and Frames, 9(2), 178–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Steels, L., & Loetzsch, M.
(2012) The Grounded Naming Game. In L. Steels (Ed.), Experiments in Cultural Language Evolution (pp. 41–59). Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Steen, F. F., & Turner, M. B.
(2013) Multimodal Construction Grammar. In M. Borkent, B. Dancygier, & J. Hinell (Eds.), Language and the Creative Mind (pp. 255–274). Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Stegmann, U. E.
(Ed.) (2013) Animal Communication Theory: Information and Influence (1 edition). Cambridge, UK; New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, M.
(1990) The view of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4). 758–759. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, T. N.
(2012) Referential mobbing calls elicit different predator-searching behaviours in Japanese great tits. Animal Behaviour, 84(1), 53–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Alarm calls evoke a visual search image of a predator in birds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(7), 1541–1545. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, T. N., Wheatcroft, D., & Griesser, M.
(2020) The syntax–semantics interface in animal vocal communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375(1789) 20180405. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tallerman, M.
(2012) Protolanguage. In M. Tallerman & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution (pp. 479–491). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tallerman, M., & Gibson, K. R.
(2012) Introduction: The evolution of language. In M. Tallerman & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution (pp. 1–35). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Templin, M.
(1957) Certain Language Skills in Children. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logo
ten Cate, C.
(2017) Assessing the uniqueness of language: Animal grammatical abilities take center stage. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(1), 91–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
ten Cate, C., & Petkov, C. I.
(2019) The Grammatical Abilities of Animals: A Comparative Overview. In P. Hagoort (Ed.), Human Language: From Genes and Brains to Behavior (pp. 687–700). Cambridge, MA & London, England: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Terrace, H. S., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J., & Bever, T. G.
(1979) Can an ape create a sentence? Science (New York, N.Y.) 206(4421), 891–902. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tincoff, R., & Jusczyk, P. W.
(1999) Some Beginnings of Word Comprehension in 6-Month-Olds. Psychological Science, 10(2), 172–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(1999) The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(2003) Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Harvard, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(2006) Construction Grammar For Kids. Constructions; Special Volume 1.Google Scholar
(2008) Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) The Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. In E. L. Bavin (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language (pp. 69–87). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019) Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Belknap Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Call, J., Warren, J. A., Frost, G. T., Carpenter, M., & Nagell, K. M.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H.
(2005) Understanding and Sharing Intentions: The Origins of Cultural Cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675–691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Townsend, S. W., & Manser, M. B.
(2013) Functionally Referential Communication in Mammals: The Past, Present and the Future. Ethology, 119(1), 1–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Traugott, E. C., & Trousdale, G.
(2013) Constructionalization and Constructional Changes. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Truswell, R.
(2017) Dendrophobia in Bonobo Comprehension of Spoken English. Mind & Language, 32(4), 395–415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vicente, A. & Falkum, I. L.
(2017) Polysemy. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Ed. Mark Aronoff. New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vonk, J.
(2020) Forty years on from the question of referential signals in nonhuman communication. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 7(2), 82–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Watson, S. K., Townsend, S. W., Schel, A. M., Wilke, C., Wallace, E. K., Cheng, L., … Slocombe, K. E.
(2015) Vocal learning in the functionally referential food grunts of chimpanzees. Current Biology, 25(4), 495–499. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wheeler, B. C., & Fischer, J.
(2012) Functionally referential signals: A promising paradigm whose time has passed. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 21(5), 195–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A.
(2017) Social Learning and Culture in Child and Chimpanzee. Annual Review of Psychology, 68(1), 129–154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019) Cultural Evolution in Animals. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 50(1), 27–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilke, C., Kavanagh, E., Donnellan, E., Waller, B. M., Machanda, Z. P., & Slocombe, K. E.
(2017) Production of and responses to unimodal and multimodal signals in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. Animal Behaviour, 1231, 305–316. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winters, James, Kirby, S. & Smith, K.
(2018) Contextual predictability shapes signal autonomy. Cognition 1761. 15–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yule, G.
(2016) The Study of Language 6th Edition (6 edition). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ziem, A.
(2017) Do we really need a Multimodal Construction Grammar? Linguistics Vanguard, 3(s1). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zima, E.
(2014) Gibt es multimodale Konstruktionen? Eine Studie zu [V(motion) in circles] und [all the way from X PREP Y]. Gesprächsforschung, 151, 1–48.Google Scholar
Zima, E., & Bergs, A.
(2017) Multimodality and construction grammar. Linguistics Vanguard, 3(s1). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K.
(2018) Combinatorial capacities in primates. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 211, 161–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) Syntax and compositionality in animal communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1789) 20190062. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K., Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M.
(1999) Conceptual semantics in a nonhuman primate. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113(1), 33–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 5 other publications

Amphaeris, Jenny, Graeme Shannon & Thora Tenbrink
2022. Overlap not gap: Understanding the relationship between animal communication and language with Prototype Theory. Lingua 272  pp. 103332 ff. DOI logo
Hartmann, Stefan & Michael Pleyer
2021. Constructing a protolanguage: reconstructing prehistoric languages in a usage-based construction grammar framework. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 376:1824 DOI logo
Pleyer, Michael
2023. The role of interactional and cognitive mechanisms in the evolution of (proto)language(s). Lingua 282  pp. 103458 ff. DOI logo
Pleyer, Michael & Stefan Hartmann
2024. Cognitive Linguistics and Language Evolution, DOI logo
Pleyer, Michael, Ryan Lepic & Stefan Hartmann
2022. Compositionality in Different Modalities: A View from Usage-Based Linguistics. International Journal of Primatology DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 15 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.