Article published In:
Biological Evolution: More than a metaphor for grammar change
Edited by Maria Rita Manzini
[Evolutionary Linguistic Theory 3:1] 2021
► pp. 655
References
Avgustinova, T.
(1997) Word order and clitics in Bulgarian. PhD Thesis. Saarbrücken: Universität des Saarlandes.Google Scholar
Becker, K. F.
(1845) Schulgrammatik des Deutschen. Frankfurt: Verlag von G.F. Kettembeil.Google Scholar
Brenowitz E. A.
(2008) Plasticity of the song control system in adult birds. In Ph. H. Zeigler & P. Marler (eds). Neuroscience of birdsong. 332-349. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bell, G. & Gonzalez, A.
(2011) Adaptation and evolutionary rescue in metapopulations experiencing environmental deterioration. Science, 332 (6035), 1327–1329. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Biberauer, Th. & Walkden, G.
(2015) Introduction: Changing views of syntactic change. In Th. Biberauer & G. Walkden (Eds.), Syntax over time. Lexical, morphological, and information-structural interactions (pp. 1–13). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bierwisch, M.
(2012) The Concept of Evolution in Linguistics. In A. Fasolo (Ed.), The Theory of Evolution and Its Impact (pp. 103–117). Milano: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Breslin, D.
(2010) Generalising Darwinism to study socio-cultural change. International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy, 30(7–8), 427–439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bridges, K. & Hoff, E.
(2014) Older sibling influences on the language environment and language development of toddlers in bilingual homes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(2), 225–241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brinckmann, C. & Bubenhofer, N.
(2012) Sagen kann man’s schon, nur schreiben tut man’s selten” – Die tun-Periphrase. In M. Konopka & R. Schneider, Grammatische Stolpersteine digital – Festschrift für Bruno Strecker zum 65. Geburtstag. Mannheim: Institut für deutsche Sprache. [URL]
Cable, S.
(2008) Configurationality and the Salish language. Theoretical perspectives on languages of the Pacific Northwest. Lecture notes. U. Mass at Amherst. [URL: [URL]]
Christiansen, M. & Chater, N.
(2016) The Now-or-Never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, E62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(2011) Language and other cognitive systems. What is special about language? Language Learning and Development, 7(4), 263–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Comrie, B. & Kuteva, T.
(2005) The evolution of grammatical structures and ‘functional need’ explanations. In M. Tallerman (Ed.) Language origins: perspective on evolution (pp. 185–207). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cornips, L.
(1998) Habitual doen in Heerlen Dutch. In J. A. van Leuvensteijn, I. Tieken-Boon van Ostade, M. van der Wal (Eds.), DO in English, Dutch and German – history and present-day variation (pp. 83–101). Amsterdam: Stichting Nederlandistiek/Nodus Publikationen.Google Scholar
De Boer, B., Thompson, B., Ravignani, A. & Boeckx, C.
(2020) Evolutionary dynamics do not motivate a single-mutant theory of human language. Nature Science Reports, 10, 451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Darwin, Ch.
(1872) On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London: John Murray (6th edition).Google Scholar
Dehé, N.
(2004) On the order of objects in Icelandic double object constructions. UCL Working Papers in Linguistics, 161, 85–108.Google Scholar
Dixon, R.
(1997) The rise and fall of languages. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunn, M.
(2014) Gender determined dialect variation. In: Corbett, G. G. (Ed.) The expression of gender (pp. 39–67). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Ellegård, A.
(1953) The auxiliary Do: The establishment and regulation of its use in English. (Gothenburg Studies in English, vol. 2). Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell.Google Scholar
Eldredge, N. & Gould, St. J.
(1972) Punctuated equilibria: an alternative to phyletic gradualism. In: Schopf, T. J. M. (Ed.) Models in Paleobiology (pp. 82–115). San Francisco: Freeman Cooper.Google Scholar
Feder, A. F., Kryazhimskiy, S. & Plotkin, J. B.
(2014) Identifying signatures of selection in genetic time series. Genetics, 196(2), 509–522. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fisher, R. A.
(1930) The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foote, A. &, Liu, Y. T. & Gregg, W.
(2015) Convergent evolution of the genomes of marine mammals. Nature Genetics, 47(3), 272–275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gould, St. J.
(2002): The structure of evolutionary theory. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard Univ. Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gazsi, D.
(2011) Arabic-Persian Language Contact. In: Weninger, St. (Ed.) The Semitic Languages: An International Handbook. (pp. 1015–1021). Berlin: De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E., Futrell, R., Piandadosi, St. T., Dautriche, I., Mahowald, K., Bergen, L. & Levy, R.
(2019) How efficiency shapes human language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(5), 389–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gell-Mann, M. & Ruhlen, M.
(2011) The origin and evolution of word order. PNAS, 108(42), 17290–17295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., So, W. C., Özyürek, A., & Mylander, C.
(2008) The natural order of events: How speakers of different languages represent events nonverbally. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1051, 9163–9168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goulding, J.
(2020) Virus replication. British Society for Immunology. [URL] [March 15 2020].
Gray, T. J., Reagan, A. J., Dodds, P. S. & Danforth, C. M.
(2018) English verb regularization in books and tweets. PLoS ONE, 13(12), e0209651. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenhill, S. J., Wu, C-H., Hua, X., Dunn, M., Levinson, St. C. & Gray, R. D.
(2017) Evolutionary dynamics of language systems. PNAS, 114(42), E8822–E8829. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haider, H.
(1991) Die menschliche Sprachfähigkeit – exaptiv und kognitiv opak. Kognitionswissenschaft, 21, 11–26.Google Scholar
(1999) On the survival of the fittest grammar (theory). Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 181, 216–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013): Symmetry breaking in Syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2014) The VO-OV split of Germanic languages – a T3 & V2 production. Interdisciplinary Journal for Germanic Linguistics and Semiotic Analysis, 19(1), 57–79.Google Scholar
(2015a) “Intelligent design” of grammars – a result of cognitive evolution. In: A. Adli, M. García García & G. Kaufmann (Eds.), Variation in language: System- and usage-based approaches (pp. 205–240). Berlin/New York: de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015b) Head directionality – in syntax and morphology. In: A. Fábregas, J. Mateu & M. Putnam (Eds.), Contemporary linguistic parameters (pp. 73–97). London: Bloomsbury Academic.Google Scholar
(2019a) An anthropic principle in lieu of a “Universal Grammar”. In: J. M. Brown, A. Schmidt & M. Wierzba (Eds.), Of trees and birds (pp. 363–381). Potsdam: Universitätsverlag Potsdam.Google Scholar
(2019b) On absent, expletive, and non-referential subjects. In: P. Herbeck, B. Pöll, & A. C. Wolfsgruber (Eds.), Semantic and syntactic aspects of impersonality (pp. 11–46). Hamburg: Helmut Buske Verlag.Google Scholar
(2021) “OVS” – A misnomer for SVO languages with ergative alignment. lingbuzz/005680.Google Scholar
Haider, H. & Szusich, L.
(2019) Slavic languages – “SVO” languages without SVO qualities? (in press). Theoretical Linguistics, 471. (lingbuzz/004973).Google Scholar
Haig, D.
(2007) Weismann Rules! OK? Epigenetics and the Lamarckian temptation. Biology and Philosophy, 221, 415–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hanke, D.
(2004) Teleology: the explanation that bedevils biology. In: Cornwell, J. (Ed.), Explanations: Styles of explanation in science (pp. 143–155). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, M.
(1999) Optimality and diachronic adaptation. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 181, 180–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, J. A.
(1994) A Performance Theory of Order and Constituency. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Heath, J.
(1984) Language contact and language change. Annual Review of Anthropology, 131, 367–384. Palo Alto: Annual Reviews, Inc. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hodgson, G. M.
(2013) Understanding organizational evolution: Toward a research agenda using Generalized Darwinism. Organization Studies, 34(7), 973–992. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Organizational Adaptation and Evolution: Darwinism versus Lamarckism? In: A. Grandori (Ed.), The Elgar Handbook of economic organization (pp. 157–171). Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar.Google Scholar
Hodgson, G. M. & Knudsen, Th.
(2006) Why we need a generalized Darwinism and why a generalized Darwinism is not enough. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 611, 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) In search of general evolutionary principles: Why Darwinism is too important to be left to the biologists. Journal of Bioeconomics, 101, 51–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hull, D. L.
(1988) Interactors versus vehicles. In: H. C. Plotkin (Ed.) The role of behavior in evolution (pp. 19–50). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jäger, A.
(2006) Typology of Periphrastic ‘do’-constructions. Bochum: Universitätsverlag Brock-meyer.Google Scholar
Jelinek, E. & Demers, R.
(1994) Predicates and pronominal arguments in Straits Salish. Language, 701, 697–736. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jenkin, F.
(1867) The origin of species. North British Review, 461, 277–318.Google Scholar
Jespersen, O.
(1894) Progress in language. New York: McMillan & Co.Google Scholar
Johansson, S.
(2005) Origins of Language: Constraints on hypotheses. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Joseph, B.
(2001) Review of R. M. W. Dixon, The rise and fall of languages. Journal of Linguistics, 371, 180–6.Google Scholar
Kant, I.
(1790) Kritik der Urteilskraft. Berlin & Libau: Lagarde & Friedrich.Google Scholar
Karjus, A., Blythe, R. A., Kirby, S. & Smith, K.
(2020) Challenges in detecting evolutionary forces in language change using diachronic corpora. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 5(1), 45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, P.
(2008) Universals constrain change; change results in typological generalizations. In: Good, J. (Ed.) Linguistic universals and language change (pp. 23–53). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koonin, E. V.
(2011) The logic of chance: The nature and origin of biological evolution. Upper Saddle River, New Jersey: FT Press Science.Google Scholar
Koplenig, A., Meyer, P., Wolfer, S. & Müller-Spitzer, C.
(2017) The statistical trade-off between word order and word structure – Large-scale evidence for the principle of least effort. PLOS ONE 12(3): e0173614. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lamarck de Monet de, J.-B. P. A.
(1809) Philosophie zoologique, ou, exposition des considérations relative à l’histoire naturelle des animaux. Paris: F. Savy.Google Scholar
Lasnik, H. &, Sobin, N.
(2000) The who/whom puzzle: On the preservation of an archaic feature. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 181, 343–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, N.
(2004) The neurobiology of procedural memory. In: J. H. Schumann, S. E. Crowell, N. E. Jones, N. Lee, Sara A. Schuchert, and L. A. Wood (Eds.), The neurobiology of learning: Perspectives from second language acquisition (pp. 43–73). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lenormand, Th.
(2002) Gene flow and the limits to natural selection. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 17(4), 183–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levit, G. S. & Hossfeld, U. & Witt, U.
(2011) Can Darwinism be “generalized” and of what use would this be? Journal of Evolutionary Economics, 211, Art. 545. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levy, R.
(2008) Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106(3), 1126–1177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loison, L.
(2018) Lamarckism and epigenetic inheritance: a clarification. Biology & Philosophy, 33(3–4), Art. 29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loewe, L. & Hill, W. G.
(2010) The population genetics of mutations: good, bad and indifferent. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B 3651, 1153–1167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loewe, L.
(2008) Negative selection. Nature Education, 1(1), 59.Google Scholar
LI, C.
(1967) Fundamental theorem of natural selection. Nature, 2141, 505–506. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacKenzie, I.
(2013) Participle-object agreement in French and the theory of grammatical viruses. Journal of Romance Studies, 131,19-33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Masel, J.
(2011) Genetic drift. Current Biology, 21(20), PR837–R838. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mayr, E.
(1991) One long argument. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(1992) The idea of teleology. Journal of the history of ideas, 53(1), 117–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Merkle, L.
(1975) Bairische Grammatik. München: Hugendubel.Google Scholar
Morgan-Short, K. & Ullman, M. T.
(2011) The neuro-cognition of second language. In: S. M. Gass & A. Mackey (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 282–299). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Morris, S.
(1994) Fleeming Jenkin and the origin of species: A reassessment. The British Journal for the History of Science, 27(3), 313–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, R. R.
(2007) Universal Darwinism and evolutionary social science. Biology and Philosophy, 221, 73–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nesensohn, E.-M.
(2012) Die Tun-Periphrase in der Kindersprache. Diploma Thesis Univ. Vienna. [URL]
Nettle, D.
(1999) Linguistic diversity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Newberry, M. G., Ahern, Ch. A., Clark, R. & Plotkin, J. B.
(2017) Detecting evolutionary forces in language change. Nature, 551(7679), 223–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nichols, J.
(1992) Linguistic diversity in space and time. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Orr, H. A.
(2009) Fitness and its role in evolutionary genetics. Nature Reviews Genetics, 101, 531–539. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oshima-Takane, Y., Goodz, E. & Derevensky, J. L.
(1996) Birth order effects on early language development: Do second born children learn from overheard speech? Child Development, 67(2), 621–634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, M.
Peters, P.
(2004) The Cambridge guide to English usage. Cambridge , UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, St. & Bloom, P.
(1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 131, 707–784. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reesink, G., Singer, R. & Dunn, M.
(2009) Explaining the linguistic diversity of Sahul using population models. PLoS Biololgy, 7(11), e1000241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rispoli, M., Hadley, P. & Holt, J.
(2012) Sequence and system in the acquisition of tense and agreement. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 551, 1007–1021. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roossinck, M. J.
(2011) The good viruses: viral mutualistic symbioses. Nature Review of Microbiology, 91, 99–108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roossinck, M. J. & Bazán, E. R.
(2017) Symbiosis: viruses as intimate partners. Annual Review of Virology, 4(1), 123–139. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rubio, L., Guerri, J. & Moreno, P.
(2013) Genetic variability and evolutionary dynamics of viruses of the family Closteroviridae. Frontiers in Microbiology, 4:151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sapir, E.
(1921) Language an Introduction to the Study of Speech. New York: Harcourt, Brace & Co.Google Scholar
Schleicher, A.
(1873) Die Darwinsche Theorie und die Sprachwissenschaft. Weimar: H. Böhlau.Google Scholar
Schoenemann, P. Th.
(2012) Evolution of brain and language. In: M. A. Hofman & D. Falk (Eds.), Progress in Brain Research 1951 (pp. 443–459). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Schütze, C.
(1999) English expletive constructions are not infected. Linguistic Inquiry, 30(3), 467–484. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siewierska, A.
(1996) Word order type and alignment type. STUF – Language Typology and Universals, 49(2), 149–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siewierska, A. & Uhliřová, L.
(2010) An overview of word order in Slavic languages. In: Siewierska, A. (Ed.) Constituent order in the languages of Europe (pp. 105–150). Berlin: Mouton DeGruyter.Google Scholar
Simon, H.
(1996) The sciences of the artificial. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Skipper, R. A. Jr. & Dietrich, M. R.
(2012) Sewall Wright’s adaptive landscape: philosophical reflections on heuristic value. In: E. Svensson & R. Calsbeek (Eds.), The adaptive landscape in evolutionary biology (pp. 17–25). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sobin, N.
(1997) Agreement, default rules, and grammatical viruses. Linguistic Inquiry, 28(2), 318–343.Google Scholar
Solan, Z., Ruppin, E., Horn, D. & Edelman, S.
(2005) Evolution of language diversity: why fitness counts. In: M. Tallerman (Ed.), Language origins: perspective on evolution (pp. 357–371). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Spielman, St. J., Weaver, St., Shank, St. D., Magalis, B. R., Li, M. & Kosakovsky Pond, S. L.
(2019) Evolution of viral genomes: Interplay between selection, recombination, and other forces. In: M. Anisimova (Ed.) Evolutionary Genomics: Statistical and Computational Methods (pp. 427–468). New York: Humana. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spocter M.A., Hopkins, W.D, Garrison, A.R, Bauernfeind, A.L., Stimpson, C.D., Hof, P.R. & Sherwood C.C.
(2010) Wernicke’s area homologue in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and its relation to the appearance of modern human language. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277 (1691), 2165-2174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stringer, Ch. & Galway-Witham, J.
(2017) On the origin of our species. Nature, 5461, 212–214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stromswold, K.
(1990) Learnability and the acquisition of auxiliaries. PhD Thesis. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Sundquist, J. D.
(2012) Negative movement in the history of Norwegian: the evolution of a grammatical virus. In: D. Jonas & J. Whitman (Eds.), Grammatical change: origins, nature, outcomes (pp. 293–312). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Szyf, M.
(2014) Lamarck revisited: epigenetic inheritance of ancestral odor fear conditioning. Nature Neuroscience, 171, 2–4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Theißen, G.
(2009) Saltational evolution: hopeful monsters are here to stay. Theory in Biosciences, 1281, 43–51.) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thomason, S. G. & Kaufman, T.
(1988) Language contact, creolization and genetic linguistics. Berkeley: University of California Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T.
(2001) A neurocognitive perspective on language: The declarative/procedural model. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 21, 717–726. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vermeer, A.
(2001) Breadth and depth of vocabulary in relation to L1/L2 acquisition and frequency of input. Applied Psycholinguistics, 221, 217–234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Voss, P., Thomas, M. E., Cisneros-Franco, J. M. & de Villers-Sidani, É.
(2017) Dynamic brains and the changing rules of neuroplasticity: Implications for learning and recovery. Frontiers in Psychology, 81, 1657. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wahlström, M.
(2015) Loss of case inflection in Bulgarian and Macedonian. Doctoral Diss. Helsinki: University of Helsinki, Department of Modern Languages.Google Scholar
Winford, D.
(2003) An Introduction to Contact Linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Witt, U.
(2006) Evolutionary concepts in economics and biology. Journal of Evolutionary Economics, 16(5), 473–476. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wright, S.
(1932) The roles of mutation, inbreeding, crossbreeding and selection in evolution. Proceedings of the 6th Int. Congress on Genetics 11, 356–366.Google Scholar