Commentary in:
Biological Evolution: More than a metaphor for grammar change
Edited by Maria Rita Manzini
[Evolutionary Linguistic Theory 3:1] 2021
► pp. 93108
References

References

Arias-Trejo, N. & Plunkett, K.
(2009): Lexical-semantic priming effects during infancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364(1536),3633–3647. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Axelsen, J. B. & Manrubia, S.
(2014): River density and landscape roughness are universal determinants of linguistic diversity. Proceedings of the Royal Society Biology, 281(1788), Article 20133029. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bentz, C., Dediu, D., Verkerk, A. & Jäger, G.
(2018): The evolution of language families is shaped by the environment beyond neutral drift. Nature Human Behavior, 2(11), 816–831. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D.
(1983): Creole languages. Scientific American, 249(1), 116–123. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014): More than nature needs: Language, mind, and evolution. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., Perkins, R. & Pagliuca, W.
(1994): The evolution of grammar: Tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago & London: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C.
(1868): The variation of animals and plants under domestication. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
(1871): The descent of man, and delection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R.
(2006): The blind watchmaker: Why the evidence of evolution reveals a universe without design. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Dediu, D. & Ladd, R.
(2007): Linguistic tone is related to the population frequency of the adaptive haplogroups of two brain size genes, ASPM and Microcephalin . PNAS, 104(26), 10944–10949. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Eldredge, N. & Gould, S. J.
(1972): Punctuated equilibria: an alternative to phyletic gradualism. In T. J. M. Schopf (Ed.), Models in paleobiology (pp. 82–115). San Francisco, CA: Freeman, Cooper & Company.Google Scholar
Ferrand, L. & New, B.
(2003): Semantic and associative priming in the mental lexicon. In P. Bonin (Ed.), Mental lexicon: Some words to talk about (pp. 25–43). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publisher.Google Scholar
Gavin, M. C., Botero, C. A., Bowern, C., Colwell, R. K., Dunn, M., Gray, D. R., Kirby, K. R., McCarter, J., Powell, A., Rangel, Th. F., Stepp, J. R., Trautwein, M., Verdolin, J. L., Yanega, G.
(2013): Towards a mechanistic understanding of linguistic diversity. Bioscience, 63(7), 524–535. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gavin, M. C., Rangel, T. F., Bowern, C., Colwell, R. K., Kirby, K. R., Botero, C. A., Dunn, M., Dunn, R. R., McCarter, J., Pacheco Coelho, M. T., Gray, R. D.
(2017): Process-based modelling shows how climate and demography shape language diversity. Global Ecology and Biogeography, 26(5), 584–591. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T.
(2009): The genesis of syntactic complexity. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gould, S. J.
(2002): The Structure of Evolutionary Theory. Cambridge: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Greenhill, S. J., Wu, C.-H., Dunn, M., Levinson, S. C. & Gray, R. D.
(2017): Evolutionary dynamics of language systems. PNAS, 114(42), E8822–E8829. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jäger, G.
in press): Can language evolution lead change for the worse? In D. W. Enke, L. M. Hyman, J. Nichols, G. Seiler & Th. Weber Eds. Language change for the worse Berlin Language Science Press https://​langsci​-press​.org​/catalog​/book​/292
Keeling, P. J. & Palmer, J. D.
(2008): Horizontal gene transfer in eukaryotic evolution. Nature Reviews Genetics, 9(8), 605–618. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leiss, E.
(2000): Artikel und Aspekt: Die grammatischen Muster von Definitheit. Berlin, New York: Walter de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010): Koverter Abbau des Artikels im Deutschen. In D. Bittner & L. Gaeta (Eds.), Kodierungstechniken im Wandel: Das Zusammenspiel von Analytik und Synthese im Gegenwartsdeutschen (pp. 137–157). Berlin, New York: Walter de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leiss, E. & Abraham, W.
(2020): The weakness of the definite article in German. In M. Tanaka, T. Tsutsui & M. Hashimoto (Eds.), Linguistic research as an interdisciplinary science. Festschrift für Mitsunobu Yoshida (pp. 77–93). Tokyo: Hituzi Publishers.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. & Dediu, D.
(2013): The interplay of genetic and cultural factors in ongoing language evolution. In P. J. Richerson & M. H. Christiansen (Eds.), Cultural evolution. Society, technology, language, and religion (pp. 219–232). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C. & Gray, R. D.
(2012): Tools from evolutionary biology shed light on the diversification of languages. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 167–173. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, E.
(1972): Lamarck revisited. Journal of the History of Biology, 5(1), 66–94. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mikołajczak-Matyja, N.
(2015): The associative structure of the mental lexicon: hierarchical semantic relations in the minds of blind and sighted language users. Psychology of Language and Communication, 19(1), 1–18. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ochman, H., Lawrence, J. G. & Groisman, E. A.
(2000): Lateral gene transfer and the nature of bacterial innovation. Nature, 405(6784), 299–304. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rämä, P., Sirri, L. & Serres, J.
(2013): Development of lexical-semantic language system: N400 priming effect for spoken words in 18- and 24-month old children. Brain and Language, 125(1), 1–10. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sandler, W., Meir, I., Padden, C. & Aronoff, M.
(2005): The emergence of grammar: systematic structure in a new language. PNAS, 102(7), 2661–2665. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sapolsky, R.
(2017): Behave. The Biology of Humans at Our Best and Worst. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Senghas, A., Kita, S. & Ozyürek, A.
(2004): Children creating core properties of language: evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua. Science, 305(5691), 1779–1782. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shaumyan, S.
(1987): A semiotic theory of language. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K.
(2007): Dawkins vs. Gould. Survival of the fittest. London: Icon Books.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T.
(2001): A neurocognitive perspective on language: The declarative/procedural model. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(10), 717–726. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wong, P. C. M., Chandrasekaran, B. & Zheng, J.
(2012): The derived allele of ASPM is associated with lexical tone perception. PLOS ONE, 7(4), e34243. CrossrefGoogle Scholar