Article published In:
Autism, Language, Communication and Cognition
Edited by Stephanie Durrleman
[Evolutionary Linguistic Theory 4:1] 2022
► pp. 129151
References
Alcántara, J. I., Weisblatt, E. J. L., Moore, B. C. J., & Bolton, P. F.
(2004) Speech-in-noise perception in high-functioning individuals with autism or Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 45 (6), 1107–1114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association
(2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andrés-Roqueta, C. and Katsos, N.
(2017) The Contribution of Grammar, Vocabulary and Theory of Mind in Pragmatic Language Competence in Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders. Front. Psychol. 8:996. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arunachalam, S. & R. J. Luyster
(2016) The Integrity of Lexical Acquisition Mechanisms in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Research Review. Autism Res 2016, 91: 810–828. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baldo, J. V., Bunge, S. A., Wilson, S. M., & Dronkers, N. F.
(2010) Is relational reasoning dependent on language? A voxel-based lesion symptom mapping study. Brain and Language, 113 (2), 59–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baldo, J. V., Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D., Ludy, C., Raskin, P., & Kim, J.
(2005) Is problem solving dependent on language? Brain and Language, 92 (3), 240–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Banney, R. M., Harper-Hill, K., & Arnott, W. L.
(2015) The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule and narrative assessment: Evidence for specific narrative impairments in autism spectrum disorders. International journal of speech-language pathology, 17 (2), 159–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., A. M. Leslie, U. Frith
(1985) Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?, Cognition 21:1, pp. 37–46, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bartak, L., Rutter, M. & Cox, A.
(1975) A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder. I. The children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1261, 127–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1977) A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder. III. Discriminant functions analysis. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 71, 383–396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bartolucci, G., S. Pierce, & D. Streiner
(1980) Cross-sectional studies of grammatical morphemes in autistic and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 10(I). 39–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D.
(2010) Overlaps between autism and language impairment: Phenomimicry or shared etiology. Behavior Genetics 401. 618–629. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K., Russell, A., & Wassenberg, K.
(1987) Turn taking affects the quality of infant vocalizations. Journal of Child Language, 14(2), 211–227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boucher, J.
(2012a) Structural language in autistic spectrum disorder – Characteristics and causes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(3), 219–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012b) Putting theory of mind in its place: psychological explanations of the socio-emotional-communicative impairments in autistic spectrum disorder. Autism 2012 May;16(3):226–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bourguignon, N., Nadig, A., and Valois, D.
(2012) The biolinguistics of autism: Emergent perspectives. Biolinguistics 61: 124–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brock, J.
(2007) Language abilities in Williams syndrome: A critical review. Development and Psychopathology, 191, 97–127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bryson, S. E., Clark, B. S., & Smith, T. M.
(1988) First report of a Canadian epidemiological study of autistic syndromes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 291, 433–445. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Butterfill, S A. & I. Apperly
(2013) How to Construct a Minimal Theory of Mind. Mind Lang, 281: 606–637. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
(2014) Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years–autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 63(SS02), 1–21.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1992) Explaining language use. Philosophical Topics 20–11, pp. 205–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. J. M., Koster, I., & Noom, M. J.
(2010) The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 30(4), 352–366. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corr, A.
(2021) The grammar of the utterance: how to do things with Ibero-Romance. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dominguez, S., Devouche, E., Apter, G., & Gratier, M.
(2016) The Roots of Turn-Taking in the Neonatal Period. Infant and Child Development, 25(3), 240–255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Durrleman, S., Burnel, M., Thommen, E., Foudon, N., Sonie, S., Reboul, A., et al.
(2016) The language-cognition interface in ASD: Complement sentences and false belief reasoning. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 211, 109–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Durrleman, S., Hinzen, W., Franck, J.
(2018) False belief and relative clauses in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Commun Disord. 741:35–44. DOI logo. Epub 2018 Apr 17. PMID: 29753216.Google Scholar
Eigsti, I.-M., Bennetto, L. & Dadlani, M.
(2007) Beyond pragmatics: Morphosyntactic development in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 371, 1007–1023. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., Kover, S. T.
(2015) Preschool language variation, growth, and predictors in children on the autism spectrum. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 56(12):1327–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eyler, L. T., Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E.
(2012) A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism. Brain, 135 (3), 949–960. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farrar, J. M., Benigno, J. P., Tompkins, V., & Gage, N. A.
(2017) Are there different pathways to explicit false belief understanding? General language and complementation in typical and atypical children. Cognitive Development, 431, 49–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fay, W. & Schuler, A.
(1980) Emerging language in autistic children. Baltimore: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Ferstl, E. C., Neumann, J., Bogler, C., & von Cramon, D. Y.
(2008) The extended language network: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension. Human Brain Mapping 29(5), 581–593. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fine, J., Bartolucci, G., Szatmari, P., & Ginsberg, G.
(1994) Cohesive discourse in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 241, 315–329. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fletcher-Watson, S., McConnell, F., Manola, E., McConachie, H.
(2014) Interventions based on the Theory of Mind cognitive model for autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2014(3):CD008785. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Interventions based on the theory of mind cognitive model for autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (3):CD008785. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fonseca, J., Raposo, A., & Martins, I. P.
(2018) Cognitive functioning in chronic post-stroke aphasia. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult.Google Scholar
Foss-Feig, J. H., Schauder, K. B., Key, A. P., Wallace, M. T., & Stone, W. L.
(2017) Audition-specific temporal processing deficits associated with language function in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 10 (11), 1845–1856. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frankland, S. M., Greene, J. D.
(2020) Concepts and Compositionality: In Search of the Brain’s Language of Thought. Annu Rev Psychol. 4;71:273–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frith, U.
(1989) Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwells.Google Scholar
Garrido, D., Carballo, G., Franco, V., & García-Retamero, R.
(2015) Dificultades de comprensión del lenguaje en niños no verbales con trastornos del espectro autista y sus implicaciones en la calidad de vida familiar. Rev Neurol. 60(4):207–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A., & Abbot-Smith, K.
(2021) The Constructionist Approach Offers a Useful Lens on Language Learning in Autistic Individuals. Language, to appear. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gratier, M., Devouche, E., Guellai, B., Infanti, R., Yilmaz, E., & Parlato-Oliveira, E.
(2015) Early development of turn-taking in vocal interaction between mothers and infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 61, 1–10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Happé, F. and Frith, U.
(2020) Annual Research Review: Looking back to look forward – changes in the concept of autism and implications for future research. J Child Psychol Psychiatr, 611: 218–232. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N.
(1970) Psychological experiments with autistic children. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Heyes, C.
(2014) False belief in infancy: a fresh look. Dev Sci. 17(5):647–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C., D. Bang, N. Shea, C. D. Frith, & S. M. Fleming
(2020) Knowing Ourselves Together: The Cultural Origins of Metacognition, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 24:5, pp. 349–362, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hinzen, W., & M. Sheehan
(2015) The philosophy of universal grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hinzen, W.
(2017) Reference across pathologies: a new linguistic lens on disorders of thought. Theoretical Linguistics 43 (3–4), 169–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hinzen, W., D. Slušná, K. Schroeder, G. Sevilla & E. Vila
(2019) Mind – Language = ? The significance of nonverbal autism. Mind & Language, 1–25.Google Scholar
Hinzen, W. & M. Wiltschko
(2022) Modelling linguistic variation in cognitive disorders. Journal of Linguistics, pp. 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Howlin, P., Savage, S., Moss, P., Tempier, A., & Rutter, M.
(2014) Cognitive and language skills in adults with autism: A 40-year follow-up. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 55 (1), 49–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsu, H. C., Fogel, A., & Messinger, D. S.
(2001) Infant non-distress vocalization during mother-infant face-to-face interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 24(1), 107–128. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hula, W. D., & McNeil, M. R.
(2008) Models of attention and dual-task performance as explanatory constructs in aphasia. Seminars in speech and language, 29 (3), 169–4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hus Bal, V., Katz, T., Bishop, S. L. & Krasileva, K.
(2016) Understanding definitions of minimally verbal across instruments: Evidence for subgroups within minimally verbal children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 571, 1424–1433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2005) Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16 (5), 367–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jack, A., & A. Pelphrey, K.
(2017) Understudied populations within the autism spectrum – current trends and future directions in neuroimaging research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 58 (4), 411–435. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kanner, L.
1943Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child 21.217–250.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., Berthoud, I., Davies, M., Howlin, P., Udwin, O.
(1997) Language and Williams syndrome: how intact is “intact”? Child Dev. 68(2):246–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kiran, S., Meier, E. L., Kapse, K. J., & Glynn, P. A.
(2015) Changes in task-based effective connectivity in language networks following rehabilitation in post-stroke patients with aphasia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9 1(June), 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kissine, M., X. Luffin, F. Aiad; R. Bourourou, G. Deliens, & N. Gaddour
(2019) Non-colloquial Arabic in Tunisian children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Language Learning 691.44–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kissine, M.
(2021a) Autism, constructionism, and nativism. Language 97 ( 3 ), e139–e160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021b) Facing the complexity of Language in autism (Response to commentators). Language 97 (3), e228–e237. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kjelgaard, M. M. & Tager-Flusberg, H.
(2001) An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language & Cognitive Processes, 16(2–3), 287–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kohnert, K., & Windsor, J.
(2004) The search for common ground: Part II. Nonlinguistic performance by linguistically diverse learners. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 471: 891–903. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kovács, Á., T. Tauzin, E. Téglás, Gergely, G., & Csibra, G.
(2014) Pointing as epistemic request: 12 month-olds point to receive new information. Infancy 19(6). 543–557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Munson, J., Estes, A., Dawson, G.
(2013) Brain Responses to Words in 2-Year-Olds with Autism Predict Developmental Outcomes at Age 6. PLoS ONE 8(5): e64967. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kulke, L., M. Reiß, H. Krist, H. Rakoczy
(2018) How robust are anticipatory looking measures of Theory of Mind? Replication attempts across the life span, Cognitive Development 461, pp. 97–111, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kulke, L., M. Wübker, H. Rakoczy
(2019) Is implicit Theory of Mind real but hard to detect? Testing adults with different stimulus materials. R. Soc. open sci. 6190068190068, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B., S. Ellis Weismer, C. A. Miller, D. A. Francis, J. Tomblin, R. Kail
(2007) Speed of Processing, Working Memory, and Language Impairment in Children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 501, 408–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lin, N., Yang, X., Li, J. et al.
Neural correlates of three cognitive processes involved in theory of mind and discourse comprehension. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 181, 273–283 (2018) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lombardo, M. V., K. Pierce, L. T. Eyler, C. Carter Barnes, C. Ahrens-Barbeau, S. Solso, K. Campbell, Eric Courchesne
(2015) Different Functional Neural Substrates for Good and Poor Language Outcome in Autism, Neuron 86:2, 567–577. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maljaars, J., Noens, I., Jansen, R., Scholte, E., & van Berckelaer-Onnes, I.
(2011) Intentional communication in nonverbal and verbal low-functioning children with autism. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44 (6), 601–614. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maljaars, J., I. Noens, E. Scholte & I. van Berckelaer-Onnes
(2012) Language in Low-Functioning Children with Autistic Disorder: Differences Between Receptive and Expressive Skills and Concurrent Predictors of Language. J Autism Dev Disord 421:2181–2191. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marno, H., T. Farroni, Y. Vidal Dos Santos, M. Ekramnia, M. Nespor, & Mehler, J.
(2015) Can you see what I am talking about. Human speech triggers referential expectations in four month-old infants. Nature Scientific Reports 51. 13594. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, T., W. Hinzen
2014The grammar of the essential indexical. Lingua 1481, 95–117, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, G. E., J. Klusek, B. Estigarribia, and J. E. Roberts
(2009) Language Characteristics of Individuals with Down Syndrome. Top Lang Disord. 29(2): 112–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B. & Becerra, A. M.
(2007) Language and communicative development in Williams syndrome. Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews 131, 3–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mills, B., Lai, J., Brown, T. T., Erhart, M., Halgren, E., Reilly, J., … Moses, P.
(2015) White Matter Microstructure Correlates of Narrative Production in Typically Developing Children and Children with High Functioning Autism Brian. Neuropsychologia, 51 (10), 1933–1941. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mizuno, A., Liu, Y., Williams, D., Keller, T., Minshew, N. & Just, M.
(2011) The neural basis of deictic shifting in linguistic perspective-taking in high-functioning autism. Brain, 1341, 2422–2435. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Modyanova, N. N.
(2009) Semantic and pragmatic language development in typical acquisition, autism spectrum disorders, and Williams syndrome with reference to developmental neurogenetics of the latter (Doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology).
Moseley, R. L., Correia, M. M., Baron-Cohen, S., Shtyrov, Y., Pulvermüller, F. & Mohr, B.
(2016) Reduced volume of the arcuate fasciculus in adults with high-functioning autism spectrum conditions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 101, 214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D. V.
(2003) Narrative skills of children with communication impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(3), 287–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norrelgen, F., Fernell, E., Eriksson, M., Hedvall, Å., Persson, C., Sjölin, M., … Kjellmer, L.
(2015) Children with autism spectrum disorders who do not develop phrase speech in the preschool years. Autism, 19 (8), 934–943. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noterdaeme, M., Wriedt, E. & Höhne, C.
(2010) Asperger’s syndrome and high-functioning autism: Language, motor and cognitive profiles. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(6), 475–481. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Patten, E., Belardi, K., Baranek, G. T., Watson, L. R., Labban, J. D., & Oller, D. K.
(2014) Vocal patterns in infants with autism spectrum disorder: Canonical babbling status and vocalization frequency. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44 (10), 2413–2428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perszyk, D. R., Waxman, S. R.
(2018) Linking Language and Cognition in Infancy. Annu Rev Psychol. Jan 4;691:231–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1994) The Language Instinct. New York: HarperCollins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997) How the Mind Works. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G.
(1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and brain sciences 1(4), 515–526. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Radulescu, E. & Critchley, H. D.
(2013) Abnormalities in fronto-striatal connectivity within language networks relate to differences in grey-matter heterogeneity in Asperger syndrome. NeuroImage: Clinical, 21, 716–726. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramage, A. E., Aytur, S., & Ballard, K. J.
(2020) Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging connectivity between semantic and phonological regions of interest may inform language targets in aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 63 1(September). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reindal, L., Nærland, T., Weidle, B., Lydersen, S., Andreassen, O. A., Sund, A. M.
(2021) Structural and Pragmatic Language Impairments in Children Evaluated for Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). J Autism Dev Disord. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rødgaard, E., Jensen, K., Vergnes, J., Soulières, I., Mottron, L.
Temporal Changes in Effect Sizes of Studies Comparing Individuals With and Without Autism: A Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 2019;76(11):1124–1132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rumpf, A. L., Kamp-Becker, I., Becker, K., & Kauschke, C.
(2012) Narrative competence and internal state language of children with Asperger Syndrome and ADHD. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(5), 1395–1407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M.
(1978) Language Disorder and Infantile Autism. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism – A reappraisal of concepts and treatment (pp. 85–104). New York: Plenum Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saxe, R., Carey, S., Kanwisher, N.
(2004) Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 551:87–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schaeffer, J.
(2018) Linguistic and Cognitive Abilities in Children with Specific Language Impairment as Compared to Children with High-Functioning Autism, Language Acquisition, 25:1, 5–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schroeder, K.
(2019) Comprehension and production of referential expressions across Autism Spectrum Conditions. PhD thesis, Universitat de Barcelona.
Schroeder, K., S. Durrleman, D. Cokal, Sanfeliu, A. Masana, & W. Hinzen
(2021) Relations between intensionality, theory of mind and complex syntax in autism spectrum conditions, Cognitive Development, 591, 101071. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sheehan, M., & Hinzen, W.
(2011) Moving towards the edge. Linguistic Analysis 31, 3–41: 405–458. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shield, A., Meier, R. P., Tager-Flusberg, H.
(2015) The Use of Sign Language Pronouns by Native-Signing Children with Autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 45(7):2128–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sigman, M., McGovern, C. W.
(2005) Improvement in Cognitive and Language Skills from Preschool to Adolescence in Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 351, 15–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silleresi, S., Prévost, P., Zebib, R., Bonnet-Brilhault, F., Conte, D., Tuller, L.
(2020) Identifying Language and Cognitive Profiles in Children With ASD via a Cluster Analysis Exploration: Implications for the New ICD-11. Autism Res. 13(7):1155–1167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slušná, D., A. Rodriguez, B. Salvado, A. Vicente, & W. Hinzen
(2021) Relations between language, non-verbal cognition and conceptualization in non- or minimally verbal individuals with ASD across the lifespan. Submitted. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, N., & I. Tsimpli
(1995) The Mind of a Savant. Language learning and modularity. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stefaniak, J. D., Halai, A. D., & Lambon Ralph, M. A.
(2020) The neural and neurocomputational bases of recovery from post-stroke aphasia. Nature Reviews Neurology, 16 (1), 43–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stigler, K. A., McDonald, B. C., Anand, A., Saykin, A. J., & McDougle, C. J.
(2011) Structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging of autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research, 1380 1, 146–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tamir, D. I., Bricker, A. B., Dodell-Feder, D., Mitchell, J. P.
(2015) Reading fiction and reading minds: the role of simulation in the default network. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11(2):215–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taylor, L. J., Maybery, M. T., Whitehouse, A. J.
(2012) Do Children with Specific Language Impairment have a Cognitive Profile Reminiscent of Autism? A Review of the Literature. J Autism Dev Disord. 42(10):2067–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tek, S., Jaffery, G., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R.
(2008) Do children with autism spectrum disorders show a shape bias in word learning? Autism Research, 1 (4), 208–222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2005) Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Boston: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Call, J.
(2018) Thirty years of great ape gestures. Animal Cognition, 1–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vouloumanos, A., & Curtin, S.
(2014) Foundational tuning: How infants’ attention to speech predicts language development. Cognitive Science, 38 (8), 1675–1686. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wiltschko, M.
(2021) The grammar of interactional language. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar