References
Albert, S., & Ruiter, J. P. de
(2018) Repair: The Interface Between Interaction and Cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 10 (2), 279–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bar-On, D.
(2013) Origins of meaning: Must we ‘go Gricean’?. Mind & Language, 28 (3), 342–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bar-On, D., & Moore, R.
(2018) Pragmatic interpretation and signaler-receiver asymmetries in animal communication. In K. Andrews & J. Beck (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of philosophy of animal minds (pp. 291–300). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Barr, D. J., & Keysar, B.
(2005) Making sense of how we make sense: The paradox of egocentrism in language use. In H. L. Colston & A. N. Katz (Eds.), Figurative language comprehension: Social and cultural influences, (pp. 2141). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C.
(2005) Enzymatic computation and cognitive modularity. Mind & language, 20 (3), 259–287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bauman, R., & Sherzer, J.
(Eds.) (1989) Explorations in the Ethnography of Speaking (No. 8). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W., Cartmill, E., Genty, E., Graham, K. E., Hobaiter, C., & Tanner, J.
(2017) Great Ape Gestures: Intentional Communication with a Rich Set of Innate Signals. Animal Cognition 20 (4), 755–769. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P.
(2009) Mindreading underlies metacognition. Behavioral and brain sciences, 32 (2), 164–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) The opacity of mind: An integrative theory of self-knowledge. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021) Explicit nonconceptual metacognition. Philosophical Studies, 178 (7), 2337–2356. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P., & Ritchie, J. B.
(2012) The emergence of metacognition: Affect and uncertainty in animals. In M. J. Beran, J. L. Brandl, J. Perner & J. Proust (Eds.) Foundations of metacognition (pp.76–93). New York/Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chambres, P., Izaute, M., & Marescaux, P. J.
(Eds.) (2002) Metacognition: Process, function, and use. Springer Science & Business Media. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, A.
(2006) Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. Trends in cognitive sciences, 10 (8), 370–374. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H.
(1996) Using language. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R. & Zuberbühler, K.
(2012) Wild Chimpanzees Inform Ignorant Group Members of Danger. Current Biology 22 1, 142–146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M. & Zuberbühler, K.
(2017) Vocalizing in Chimpanzees Is Influenced by Social-Cognitive Processes. Science Advances 3 (11), e1701742. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M.
(2020) Resource-rationality beyond individual minds: The case of interactive language use. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 43 1, 23–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M., Roberts, S. G., Baranova, J., Blythe, J., Drew, P., Floyd, S., … & Enfield, N. J.
(2015) Universal principles in the repair of communication problems. PloS one, 10 (9), e0136100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M.
(1998) Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dunlosky, J., & Metcalfe, J.
(2009) Metacognition. Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Enfield, N. J.
(2017) How we talk: The inner workings of conversation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Enfield, N. J., & Sidnell, J.
(2022) Consequences of language: from primary to enhanced intersubjectivity. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Enfield, N. J., Dingemanse, M., Baranova, J., Blythe, J., Brown, P., Dirksmeyer, T., … & Torreira, F.
(2013) Huh? What?–A first survey in 21 languages. In M. Hayashi, G. Raymond & J. Sidnell (Eds.), Conversational repair and human understanding (pp. 343–380). New York/Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C.
(2009) The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and brain sciences, 32 (5), 429–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. T.
(2010) The evolution of language. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fröhlich, M., & van Schaik, C. P.
(2022) Social tolerance and interactional opportunities as drivers of gestural redoings in orang-utans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 377 (1859), 20210106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fröhlich, M., Kuchenbuch, P., Müller, G., Fruth, B., Furuichi, T., Wittig, R. & Pika, S.
(2016) Unpeeling the Layers of Language: Bonobos and Chimpanzees Engage in Cooperative Turn-taking Sequences. Scientific Reports 61, 25887. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gamble, C., Gowlett, J. & Dunbar, R.
(2014) Thinking big: How the evolution of social life shaped the human mind. Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
Gärdenfors, P.
(1995) Cued and detached representations in animal cognition. Behavioural processes, 35 (1–3), 263–273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genty, E., & Byrne, R. W.
(2010) Why Do Gorillas Make Sequences of Gestures? Animal Cognition 13 (2): 287–301. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genty, E., Breuer, T., Hobaiter, C. & Byrne, R. W.
(2009) Gestural Communication of the Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla): Repertoire, Intentionality and Possible Origins. Animal Cognition 12 (3), 527–546. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A. I.
(2006) Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. New York/Oxford: Oxford University Press on Demand. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haig, D.
(2008) Conflicting messages: genomic imprinting and internal communication. In P. D’Ettore, & D. P. Hughes (Eds.) Sociobiology of communication (pp. 209–223). New York/Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heesen, R., Fröhlich, M., Sievers, C., Woensdregt, M., & Dingemanse, M.
(2022) Coordinating social action: a primer for the cross-species investigation of communicative repair. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 377 (1859), 20210110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C., Bang, D., Shea, N., Frith, C. D., & Fleming, S. M.
(2020) Knowing ourselves together: The cultural origins of metacognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24 (5), 349–362. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hockett, F.
(1966) The Problem of Universals in Language. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of Language (pp. 1–29). Cambridge MA / London: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Holler, J., & Levinson, S. C.
(2019) Multimodal language processing in human communication. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23 (8), 639–652. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hömke, P., Holler, J., & Levinson, S. C.
(2018) Eye blinks are perceived as communicative signals in human face-to-face interaction. PloS one, 13 (12), e0208030. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Eye blinks are perceived as communicative signals in human face-to-face interaction. PloS one, 13 (12), e0208030. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, S.
(1999) Mother Nature: A History of Mothers, Infants, and Natural Selection-Sarah Blaffer Hrdy, New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B.
(2009) Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J.
(2014) Evolution in four dimensions, revised edition: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. Cambridge, MA: The MIT press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N.
(1983) Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness (No. 6). Cambridge, Ma: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A.
(1991) Beyond modularity: Innate constraints and developmental change. B. I. Bertenthal (Ed.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 171–197).Google Scholar
Keysar, B., & Barr, D. J.
(2002) Self-anchoring in conversation: Why language users do not do what they ‘should’ In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 150–166). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C.
(1983) Pragmatics. New York/Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S.
(2006) Cognition at the heart of human interaction. Discourse studies, 8(1), 85-93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C.
(2020) On the human “interaction engine”. In N. J. Enfield & S. C. Levinson, Roots of human sociality (pp. 39–69). New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
forthcoming). The Interaction Engine and the Evolution of Language.
Levinson, S. C., & Enfield, N. J.
(Eds.) (2020) Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lewis, D.
(1969) Convention: A philosophical study. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Machery, E.
(2008) Massive modularity and the flexibility of human cognition. Mind & Language, 23 (3), 263–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Metcalfe, J., & Shimamura, A. P.
(Eds.) (1994) Metacognition: Knowing about knowing. MIT press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Metcalfe, J.
(2008) Evolution of metacognition. In J. Dunlosky & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Handbook of metamemory and memory 291 (pp. 46). New York/Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Micklos, A., & Woensdregt, M.
(2022, August 19). Cognitive and Interactive Mechanisms for Mutual Understanding in Conversation. PsyArXiv. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moore, R.
(2017) Pragmatics-First Approaches to the Evolution of Language. Psychological Inquiry, 28 (2–3), 206–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Gricean communication, language development, and animal minds. Philosophy Compass, 13 (12), e12550. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, T. O.
(1992) (Ed.). Metacognition: Core Readings.Google Scholar
Nichols, S., & Stich, S. P.
(2003) Mindreading: An integrated account of pretence, selfawareness, and understanding other minds. Oxford: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicholson, T., Williams, D. M., Lind, S. E., Grainger, C., & Carruthers, P.
(2021) Linking metacognition and mindreading: Evidence from autism and dual-task investigations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150 (2), 206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oakhill, J.
(2020) Four decades of research into children’s reading comprehension: A personal review. Discourse Processes, 57 (5–6), 402–419. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peters, P., & Wong, D.
(2015) Turn management and backchannels. In K. Aijmer & C. Rühlemann (Eds.), Corpus pragmatics: A handbook (pp. 408–429). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Planer, R. J.
(2017a) How language couldn’t have evolved: a critical examination of Berwick and Chomsky’s theory of language evolution. Biology & Philosophy, 32 (6), 779–796. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017b) Protolanguage might have evolved before ostensive communication. Biological Theory, 12 (2), 72–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017c) Talking about tools: Did early pleistocene hominins have a protolanguage?. Biological Theory, 12 (4), 211–221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021) Theory of Mind, System-2 Thinking, and the Origins of Language. In A. Killin & S. Allen-Hermanson (Eds.), Explorations in Archaeology and Philosophy (pp. 171–195). Cham: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Planer, R. J., & Godfrey-Smith, P.
(2020) Communication and representation understood as sender–receiver coordination. Mind & Language 36 (5), 750–770. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Planer, R., & Sterelny, K.
(2021) From signal to symbol: the evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Proust, J.
(2003) Does metacognition necessarily involve metarepresentation?. Behavioral and brain sciences, 26(3), 352–352.Google Scholar
(2006) Rationality and metacognition in non-human animals. In S. Hurley & M. Nudds (Eds.), Rational animals (pp. 247–274). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Metacognition and metarepresentation: is a self-directed theory of mind a precondition for metacognition? Synthese, 159 (2), 271–295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Metacognition. Philosophy Compass, 5 (11), 989–998. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Metacognition and mindreading: one or two functions. Foundations of metacognition, 234–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) The evolution of primate communication and metacommunication. Mind & language, 31 (2), 177–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019) From comparative studies to interdisciplinary research on metacognition. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6 (4), 309–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rossano, F.
(2019) The structure and timing of human vs. primate social interaction. In P. Hagoort (Ed.) Human Language (pp. 201–220). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, E. A.
(1968) Sequencing in conversational openings 1. American anthropologist, 70 (6), 1075–1095. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1992) Repair after next turn: The last structurally provided defense of intersubjectivity in conversation. American journal of sociology, 97 (5), 1295–1345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) Interaction: The infrastructure for social institutions, the natural ecological niche for language, and the arena in which culture is enacted. In Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction (pp. 70–96). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A., Jefferson, G., & Sacks, H.
(1977) The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language, 53 (2), 361–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schel, A. M., Townsend, S. W., Macanda, Z., Zuberbühler, K. & Slocombe, K. E.
(2013) Chimpanzee Alarm Call Production Meets Key Criteria for Intentionality. PLoS One 8 (10): e76674. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Searcy, W. A., & Nowicki, S.
(2010) The Evolution of Animal Communication: Reliability and Deception in Signaling Systems. Princeton: Princeton University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seifart, F., Evans, N., Hammarström, H., & Levinson, S. C.
(2018) Language documentation twenty-five years on. Language, 94 (4), e324–e345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shea, N.
(2018) Metacognition and abstract concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373 (1752), 20170133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) Concept-metacognition. Mind & Language, 35 (5), 565–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shea, N., Boldt, A., Bang, D., Yeung, N., Heyes, C., & Frith, C. D.
(2014) Supra-personal cognitive control and metacognition. Trends in cognitive sciences, 18 (4), 186–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Metcalfe, J., & Shimamura, A. P.
(Eds.) (1994) Metacognition: Knowing about knowing. MIT press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T.
(2015) Speaking our minds: Why human communication is different, and how language evolved to make it special. Palgrave MacMillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skyrms, B.
(2010) Signals: Evolution, learning, and information. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soto, C., Gutiérrez de Blume, A. P., Jacovina, M., McNamara, D., Benson, N., & Riffo, B.
(2019) Reading comprehension and metacognition: The importance of inferential skills. Cogent Education, 6 (1), 1565067. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D., & Origgi, G.
(2012) A pragmatic perspective on the evolution of language. In D. Wilson & D. Sperber (Eds.) Meaning and Relevance, (pp. 331–338). Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D.
(1986) Relevance: Communication and cognition (Vol. 1421). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Spurrett, D.
(2016) Does intragenomic conflict predict intrapersonal conflict? Biology & Philosophy, 31 (3), 313–333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stalnaker, R.
(2002) Common ground. Linguistics and philosophy, 25 (5/6), 701–721. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sterelny, K.
(2012) Language, gesture, skill: the co-evolutionary foundations of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367 (1599), 2141–2151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) The evolved apprentice. Cambridge, MA: The MIT press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2003) Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(2010) Origins of human communication. MIT press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E.
(2012) Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current anthropology, 53(6), 673–692. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R.
(2000) The elements of a scientific theory of self-deception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 907 (1), 114–131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Deceit and self-deception: Fooling yourself the better to fool others. London: Penguin UK.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P.
(2016) The Primate Origins of Human Nature. Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Weinert, F. E., & Kluwe, R. H.
(1987) Metacognition, motivation, and understanding. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J.
(2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York/Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, S.
(1989) Backchannels across cultures: A study of Americans and Japanese1. Language in society, 18 (1), 59–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar