Article published In:
What can metacognition teach us about the evolution of communication?
Edited by Joëlle Proust
[Evolutionary Linguistic Theory 5:1] 2023
► pp. 79101
References
Ackerman, R., & Thompson, V. A.
(2017) Meta-Reasoning: Monitoring and Control of Thinking and Reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21 1, 607–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Astuti, R., & Harris, P. L.
(2008) Understanding Mortality and the Life of the Ancestors in Rural Madagascar. Cognitive Science, 32 1, 713–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R.
(1976) The Selfish Gene. Oxford: O.U.P.Google Scholar
Doyle, M. E., & Hourihan, K. L.
(2016) Metacognitive Monitoring During Category Learning: How Success Affects Future Behaviour. Memory, 24 1, 1197–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eglington, L. G., & Kang, S. H. K.
(2017) Interleaved Presentation Benefits Science Category Learning. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 6 1, 475–85. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eslami, SM Ali, Danilo Jimenez Rezende, Frederic Besse, Fabio Viola, Ari S Morcos, Marta Garnelo, Avraham Ruderman, Andrei A Rusu, Ivo Danihelka, and Karol Gregor
2018Neural scene representation and rendering. Science, 360 (6394), 1204–1210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernandez Cruz, A. L., Arango-Munoz, S. & Volz, K. G.
(2016) Oops, Scratch That! Monitoring One’s Own Errors During Mental Calculation. Cognition, 146 1, 110–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friston, K. J., & Stephan, K. E.
(2007) Free-Energy and the Brain. Synthese, 159 1, 417–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Godfrey-Smith, P.
(2013) Signals, Icons, and Beliefs. In D. Ryder, J. Kingsbury & K. Williford (Eds.), Millikan and Her Critics (pp. 41–58). Oxford / Malden MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Guilbeault, D., Baronchelli, A., & Centola, D.
(2021) Experimental Evidence for Scale-Induced Category Convergence across Populations. Nature Communications, 12 1, 327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamm, J. P., Johnson, B. W., & Kirk, I. J.
(2002) Comparison of the N300 and N400 Erps to Picture Stimuli in Congruent and Incongruent Contexts. Clinical neurophysiology, 113 1, 1339–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartwig, M. K., & Dunlosky, J.
(2017) Category Learning Judgments in the Classroom: Can Students Judge How Well They Know Course Topics? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 49 1, 80–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacoby, L. L., Wahlheim, C. N., & Coane, J. H.
(2010) Test-Enhanced Learning of Natural Concepts: Effects on Recognition Memory, Classification, and Metacognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36 1, 1441.Google Scholar
Jumper, J., Evans, R., Pritzel, A., Green, T., Figurnov, M., Ronneberger, O., Tunyasuvunakool, K., Bates, R., Žídek, A., & Potapenko, A.
(2021) Highly Accurate Protein Structure Prediction with Alphafold. Nature, 596 (7873), 583–589. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koriat, A.
(1997) Monitoring One’s Own Knowledge During Study: A Cue-Utilization Approach to Judgments of Learning. Journal of Experimental psychology: General, 126 1, 349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Subjective Confidence in Perceptual Judgments: A Test of the Self-Consistency Model. Journal of Experimental psychology: General, 140 1:, 117–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012a) The Self-Consistency Model of Subjective Confidence. Psychological Review, 119 1, 80–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012b) The Subjective Confidence in One’s Knowledge and Judgments: Some Metatheoretical Considerations. In M. J. Beran, J. L. Brandl, J. Perner, & J. Proust (Eds.), The Foundations of Metacognition (pp. 213–33). Oxford / New York: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012c) The Relationships between Monitoring, Regulation and Performance. Learning and Instruction, 22 1, 296–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A.
(2008) Learning Concepts and Categories: Is Spacing the “Enemy of Induction”? Psychological Science, 19 1. 585–92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kornell, N., Castel, A. D., Eich, T. S., & Bjork, R. A.
(2010) Spacing as the Friend of Both Memory and Induction in Young and Older Adults. Psychology and aging, 25 1, 498. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krizhevsky, Alex, Ilya Sutskever, and Geoffrey E Hinton
2012Imagenet classification with deep convolutional neural networks. In Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 25, edited by F. Pereira, C.J.C. Burges, L. Bottou and K.Q. Weinberger, 1097–1105. New York: Curran Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D.
(2011) Thirty Years and Counting: Finding Meaning in the N400 Component of the Event-Related Brain Potential (Erp). Annual review of psychology, 62 1, 621–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Legare, C. H., & Shtulman, A.
(2018) Explanatory Pluralism across Cultures and Development. In J. Proust & M. Fortier (Eds.), Metacogntive Diverisity: An Interdisciplinary Approach. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martinez, M.
(2013) Teleosemantics and Productivity. Philosophical Psychology, 26 1, 47–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Millikan, R. G.
(1984) Language, Thought and Other Biological Categories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1989) Biosemantics. Journal of Philosophy, 86 1, 281–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000) On Clear and Confused Ideas. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Beyond Concepts: Unicepts, Language, and Natural Information. Oxford / New York: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2022) Self-Signs and Intensional Contexts. Mind & Language: 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morehead, K., Dunlosky, J. & Foster, N. L.
(2017) Do People Use Category-Learning Judgments to Regulate Their Learning of Natural Categories? Memory & Cognition, 45 1, 1253–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L.
(1990) Metamemory: A Theoretical Framework and New Findings. In M. Bowerman (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (pp. 125–73). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press, Inc..Google Scholar
Polich, J.
(2007) Updating P300: An Integrative Theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical neurophysiology, 118 1, 2128–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Proust, J.
(2008) Epistemic Agency and Metacognition: An Externalist View. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 108 1, 241–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012a) The Norms of Acceptance. Philosophical Issues, 22 1, 316–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012b) Metacognition and Mindreading: One or Two Functions? In M. J. Beran, J. L. Brandl, J. Perner & J. Proust (Eds.), Foundations of Metacognition (pp. 234–51). Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013a) The Philosophy of Metacognition: Mental Agency and Self-Awareness. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013b) “Mental Acts as Natural Kinds”. In Vierkant, ed, Decomposing the Will. Oxford / New York: OUP, 262–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rawson, K. A., Thomas, R. C., & Jacoby, L. L.
(2015) The Power of Examples: Illustrative Examples Enhance Conceptual Learning of Declarative Concepts. Educational Psychology Review, 27 1, 483–504. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rupert, R. D.
(1999) Mental Representations and Millikan’s Theory of Intentional Content: Does Biology Chase Causality? The Southern journal of philosophy, 37 1, 113–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shea, N.
(in submission) Metacognition of Inferential Transitions.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., Clement, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., & Wilson, D.
(2010) Epistemic Vigilance. Mind & Language, 25 1, 359–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, V. A., & Johnson, S. C.
(2014) Conflict, Metacognition, and Analytic Thinking. Thinking & Reasoning, 20 1, 215–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thorne, S. R., Quilty-Dunn, J., Smortchkova, J., Shea, N., & Hampton, J. A.
(2021) Concept Appraisal. Cognitive Science, 45 1, e12978. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thorne, S. R., Smortchkova, J., Quilty-Dunn, J., Shea, N., & Hampton, J. A.
(2022) Is Concept Appraisal Modulated by Procedural or Declarative Manipulations? Frontiers in Psychology, 13 1, 774629. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wahlheim, C. N., Dunlosky, J. & Jacoby, L. L.
(2011) Spacing Enhances the Learning of Natural Concepts: An Investigation of Mechanisms, Metacognition, and Aging. Memory & Cognition, 39 1, 750–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wahlheim, C. N., Finn, B. & Jacoby, L. L.
(2012) Metacognitive Judgments of Repetition and Variability Effects in Natural Concept Learning: Evidence for Variability Neglect. Memory and Cognition, 40 1, 703–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yan, V. X., Ligon Bjork, E. & Bjork, R. A.
(2016) On the Difficulty of Mending Metacognitive Illusions: A Priori Theories, Fluency Effects, and Misattributions of the Interleaving Benefit. Journal of Experimental psychology: General, 145 1, 918. DOI logoGoogle Scholar