Review article published In:
Evolution of Communication
Vol. 2:2 (1998) ► pp.279291
References
Allen, C., & Bekoff, M.
(1995) Cognitive ethology and the intentionality of animal behavior. Mind and Language, 101, 313–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Altmann, S.
(1967) Social Communication Among Primates. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L.
(1999) The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 541, 462–479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bateson, G.
(1953) Metalogue. About games and being serious. Review of General Semantics, 101, 59–63.Google Scholar
Bekoff, M.
(1972) The development of social interaction, play, and meta-communication in mammals: An ethological perspective. Quarterly Review of Biology, 471, 412–434. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W.
(1995) The Thinking Ape. Evolutionary Origins of Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A.
(eds.) (1988) Machiavellian Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C.
(1983) Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The “Panglossian paradigm” defended. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 61, 343–390. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. M.
(1998) Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 211, 101–148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C., & Dickinson, A.
(1995) Folk psychology won’t go away: Response to Allen and Bekoff. Mind and Language, 101, 329–332. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N. K.
(1976) The social function of intellect. In P. P. G. Bateson, & R. A. Hinde (eds.), Growing Points in Ethology (pp. 303–317). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Millikan, R. G.
(1984) Language, Thought, and Other Biological Categories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1993) White Queen Psychology and Other Essays for Alice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pfungst, O.
(1911) Clever Hans, The Horse of Mr. Von Osten. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J.
(1993) Reconstructing the evolution of mind. American Psychologist, 481, 493–509. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, D.
(1996) Chimpanzee theory of mind? The long road to strong inference. In P. Carruthers & P. Smith (eds.), Theories of Theories of Mind (pp. 293–329) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G.
(1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 515–526. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, W. J.
(1977) The Behavior of Communicating. An Ethological Approach. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N.
(1963) On the aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 201, 410–433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Call, J.
(1997) Primate Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M.
(1972) A comparative approach to the phylogeny of laughter and smiling. In R. A. Hinde (ed.), Non-Verbal Communication (pp. 209–241). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M., & Wheatley, T.
(1999) Apparent mental causation. American Psychologist, 541, 480–492. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zentall, T. R.
(1999) Animal cognition: The bridge between animal learning and human cognition. Psychological Science, 101, 206–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar