References (124)
References
Anolli, L., Ciceri, R., & Infantino, M. G. (2000). Irony as a game of implicitness: Acoustic profiles of ironic communication. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 275–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002). From” blame by praise” to “praise by blame”: Analysis of vocal patterns in ironic communication. International Journal of Psychology, 37, 266–276. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aronsson, K., & Rundström, B. (1989). Cats, dogs, and sweets in the clinical negotiation of reality: On politeness and coherence in pediatric discourse. Language in Society, 18, 483–504. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attardo, S., Eisterhold, J., Hay, J., & Poggi, I. (2003). Multimodal markers of irony and sarcasm. Humor, 16, 243–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bamman, D., & Smith, N. A. (2015). Contextualized sarcasm detection on Twitter. In Proceedings of the Ninth International AAAI Conference on Web and Social Media (pp. 574–577). New York: AAAI Press.Google Scholar
Baptista, N. I., Macedo, E. C., & Boggio, P. S. (2015). Looking more and at different things: Differential gender eye-tracking patterns on an irony comprehension task. Psychology & Neuroscience, 8, 157–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Keysar, B., & Colston, H. L. (2005). Making sense of how we make sense: The paradox of egocentrism in language use. In & A. N. Katz (Eds.), Figurative language comprehension: Social and cultural influences (pp. 21–41). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bowes, A., & Katz, A. (2011). When sarcasm stings. Discourse Processes, 48, 215–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boylan, J., & Katz, A. N. (2013). Ironic expression can simultaneously enhance and dilute perception of criticism. Discourse Processes, 50, 187–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bromberek-Dyzman, K., & Rataj, K. (2016). Irony comprehension in the nonnative language comes at a cost. Psychology of Language and Communication, 20, 336–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge: Cambridge university press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruntsch, R., Hofmann, J., & Ruch, W. (2016). Virgin soil in irony research: Personality, humor, and the “sense of irony”. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 2, 25–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bryant, G. A., & Fox Tree, J. E. (2002). Recognizing verbal irony in spontaneous speech. Metaphor and Symbol, 17, 99–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005). Is there an ironic tone of voice? Language and Speech, 48, 257–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Camp, E. (2012). Sarcasm, pretense, and the semantics/pragmatics distinction. Noûs, 46, 587–634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carvalho, P., Sarmento, L., Silva, M. J., & De Oliveira, E. (2009). Clues for detecting irony in user-generated contents: oh.. .!! it’s so easy ; – ). In Proceedings of the 1st International CIKM Workshop on Topic-sentiment Analysis for Mass Opinion (pp. 53–56). New York: Association for Computing Machinery. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caucci, G. M., Kreuz, R. J., & Buder, E. H. (2008). “I’m always sincere”: What is salient in sarcasm? Paper presented at the 49th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society , Chicago.
Cheang, H. S., & Pell, M. D. (2008). The sound of sarcasm. Speech Communication, 50, 366–381. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009). Acoustic markers of sarcasm in Cantonese and English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 126, 1394–1405. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011). Recognizing sarcasm without language: A cross-linguistic study of English and Cantonese. Pragmatics & Cognition, 19, 203–223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1985). Language use and language users. In G. Lindzey, & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology, third edition (pp. 179–231). New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
(1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Gerrig, R. J. (1984). On the pretense theory of irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 121–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colston, H. L. (1997). Salting a wound or sugaring a pill: The pragmatic functions of ironic criticism. Discourse Processes, 23, 25–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002). Contrast and assimilation in verbal irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 34, 111–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015). Using figurative language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). Irony and sarcasm. In Salvatore Attardo (Ed.) The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 234–249). New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colston, H. L., & Gibbs, R. W. Jr. (2002a). Are irony and metaphor understood differently? Metaphor and Symbol, 17, 57–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002b). The risks and rewards of ironic communication. In L. Anolli, R. Ciceri, & G. Riva (Eds.), Say Not to say: New perspectives on miscommunication (pp. 181–194). Amsterdam: IOS Press.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & Keller, S. B. (1998). You’ll never believe this: Irony and hyperbole in expressing surprise. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27, 499–513. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colston, H. L., & Lee, S. Y. (2004). Gender differences in verbal irony use. Metaphor and Symbol, 19, 289–306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coupland, D. (1992, 2 November). The irony board. The New Republic, p. 12.Google Scholar
(1993, 18 January). The irony board II. The New Republic, p. 11.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. (1974). On saying what you mean without meaning what you say. In Papers from the tenth regional meeting, Chicago Linguistic Society (pp. 117–127). Chicago: Department of Linguistics, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
D’Andrade, R. G., & Wish, M. (1985). Speech act theory in quantitative research on interpersonal behavior. Discourse Processes, 8, 229–259. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davidov, D., Tsur, O., & Rappoport, A. (2010). Semi-supervised recognition of sarcastic sentences in Twitter and Amazon. In Proceedings of the Fourteenth Conference on Computational Natural Language Learning (pp. 107–116). Stroudsburg, PA: Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Derks, D., Bos, A. E. R., & von Grumbkow, J. (2008). Emoticons and online message interpretation. Social Science Computer Review, 26, 379–388. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dews, S., Kaplan, J., & Winner, E. (1995). Why not say it directly? The social functions of irony. Discourse processes, 19, 347–367. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dews, S., & Winner, E. (1995). Muting the meaning: A social function of irony. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10, 3–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dore, J. (1975). Holophrases, speech acts and language universals. Journal of Child Language, 2, 21–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dress, M. L., Kreuz, R. J., Link, K. E., & Caucci, G. M. (2008). Regional variation in the use of sarcasm. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 27, 71–85. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drucker, A., Fein, O., Bergerbest, D., & Giora, R. (2014). On sarcasm, social awareness, and gender. Humor, 27, 551–573. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ducharme, L. J. (1994). Sarcasm and interactional politics. Symbolic Interaction, 17, 51–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Filik, R., Țurcan, A., Thompson, D., Harvey, N., Davies, H., & Turner, A. (2016). Sarcasm and emoticons: Comprehension and emotional impact. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 2130–2146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garmendia, J. (2018). Irony. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., Jr. (2000). Irony in talk among friends. Metaphor and symbol, 15, 5–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., Jr., & Colston, H. L. (2012). Interpreting figurative meaning. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giora, R., Givoni, S., & Fein, O. (2015). Defaultness reigns: The case of sarcasm. Metaphor and Symbol, 30(4), 290–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
González-Fuente, S., Escandell-Vidal, V., & Prieto, P. (2015). Gestural codas pave the way to the understanding of verbal irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 90, 26–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gucman, M. (2016). The role of individual differences and situational factors in perception of verbal irony. Psychology of Language and Communication, 20, 255–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haiman, J. (1998). Talk is cheap: Sarcasm, alienation, and the evolution of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hancock, J. T. (2002). The production and use of verbal irony: An examination of expectations and the asymmetry of affect . Unpublished doctoral Dissertation. Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada.
(2004). Verbal irony use in face-to-face and computer-mediated conversations. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 23, 447–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hermann, A. (2010). The interaction of eye blinks and other prosodic cues in German Sign Language. Sign Language & Linguistics, 13, 3–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hussey, K. A., & Katz, A. N. (2006). Metaphor production in online conversation: Gender and friendship status. Discourse processes, 42, 75–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ivanko, L. S., Pexman, P. M., & Olineck, K. M. (2004). How sarcastic are you? Individual differences and verbal irony. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 23, 244–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jorgensen, J. (1996). The functions of sarcastic irony in speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 26, 613–634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Katz, A. N., Blasko, D. G., & Kazmerski, V. A. (2004). Saying what you don't mean: Social influences on sarcastic language processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 186–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kim, J. (2014). How Korean EFL learners understand sarcasm in L2 English. Journal of Pragmatics, 60, 193–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knox, N. D. (1973). Irony. In P. P. Wiener (Ed.), Dictionary of the history of ideas, volume II (pp. 626–634). New York: Scribner.Google Scholar
Kovaz, D., Kreuz, R. J., & Riordan, M. A. (2013). Distinguishing sarcasm from literal language: Evidence from books and blogging. Discourse Processes, 50, 598–615. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kreuz, R. J. (1987). An investigation into some factors influencing the perception of sarcasm: The role of echoic mention, victims, and implicit norms . Unpublished doctoral Dissertation. Princeton University, Princeton, NJ.
(1996). The use of verbal irony: Cues and constraints. In J. S. Mio, & A. N. Katz (Eds.), Metaphor: Implications and applications (pp. 23–38). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2000). The production and processing of verbal irony. Metaphor and Symbol, 15, 99–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., & Caucci, G. M. (2007). Lexical influences on the perception of sarcasm. Paper presented at the Workshop on Computational Approaches to Figurative Language, annual meeting of the Human Language Technology Conference: North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics (HLT-NAACL), Rochester, NY. DOI logo
Kreuz, R. J., & Glucksberg, S. (1989). How to be sarcastic: The echoic reminder theory of verbal irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 374–386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., Kassler, M. A., Coppenrath, L., & Allen, B. M. (1999). Tag questions and common ground effects in the perception of verbal irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 1685–1700. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., & Link, K. E. (2002). Asymmetries in the use of verbal irony. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 21, 127–143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., & Roberts, R. M. (1993). The empirical study of figurative language in literature. Poetics, 22, 151–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995). Two cues for verbal irony: Hyperbole and the ironic tone of voice. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10, 21–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). Getting through: The perils and pleasure of cross-cultural communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., Roberts, R. M., Johnson, B. K., & Bertus, E. L. (1996). Figurative language occurrence and co-occurrence in contemporary literature. In R. J. Kreuz, & M. S. MacNealy (Eds.), Empirical approaches to literature and aesthetics (pp. 83–97). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
Kreuz, R. (2020). Irony and sarcasm. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kumon-Nakamura, S., Glucksberg, S., & Brown, M. (1995). How about another piece of pie: The allusional pretense theory of discourse irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 3–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kunneman, F., Liebrecht, C., Van Mulken, M., & Van Den Bosch, A. (2015). Signaling sarcasm: From hyperbole to hashtag. Information Processing and Management, 51, 500–509. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laval, V., & Bert-Erboul, A. (2005). French-speaking children’s understanding of sarcasm: The role of intonation and context. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 610–620. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lawson, H. M., & Leck, K. (2006). Dynamics of internet dating. Social Science Computer Review, 24, 189–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liu, B. (2012). Sentiment analysis and opinion mining. San Rafael, CA: Morgan & Claypool Publishers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Louwerse, M. M., Benesh, N., Hoque, M. E., Jeuniaux, P., Lewis, G., Wu, J., & Zirnstein, M. (2007). Multimodal communication in face-to-face computer-mediated conversations. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 29 , 1235–1240.Google Scholar
Lucariello, J. (1994). Situational irony: A concept of events gone awry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 129–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mantovan, L., Giustolisi, B., & Panzeri, F. (2019). Signing something while meaning its opposite: The expression of irony in Italian Sign Language (LIS). Journal of Pragmatics, 142, 47–61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marwick, A. E., & Boyd, D. (2011). I tweet honestly, I tweet passionately: Twitter users, context collapse, and the imagined audience. New Media & Society, 13, 114–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muecke, D. C. (1978). Irony markers. Poetics, 7, 363–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Napoli, D. J., & Sutton-Spence, R. (2010). Limitations on simultaneity in sign language. Language, 86, 647–662. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The psychology of violence in the South. Boulder, CO: Westview.Google Scholar
Nunberg, G. (2001). The way we talk now: Commentaries on language and culture. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Okamoto, S. (2002). Politeness and the perception of irony: Honorifics in Japanese. Metaphor and Symbol, 17, 119–139. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Olkoniemi, H., Ranta, H., & Kaakinen, J. K. (2016). Individual differences in the processing of written sarcasm and metaphor: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 433–450.Google Scholar
Pang, B., & Lee, L. (2008). Opinion mining and sentiment analysis. Foundations and Trends in Information Retrieval, 2(1–2), 1–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pang, B., Lee, L., & Vaithyanathan, S. (2002). Thumbs up? Sentiment classification using machine learning techniques. In Proceedings of the ACL-02 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing, Volume 10 (pp. 79–86). Stroudsberg, PA: Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Peters, S., & Almor, A. (2016). Creating the sound of sarcasm. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 36, 241–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peters, S., Wilson, K., Boiteau, T. W., Gelormini-Lezama, C., & Almor, A. (2016). Do you hear it now? A native advantage for sarcasm processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 400–414. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P. M. (2008). It’s fascinating research: The cognition of verbal irony. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 286–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P. M., & Olineck, K. M. (2002). Does sarcasm always sting? Investigating the impact of ironic insults and ironic compliments. Discourse Processes, 33, 199–217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Preminger, A., & Brogan, T. V. F. (Eds.). (1993). The new Princeton encyclopedia of poetry and poetics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Rajadesingan, A., Zafarani, R., & Liu, H. (2015). Sarcasm detection on twitter: A behavioral modeling approach. In Proceedings of the Eighth ACM International Conference on Web Search and Data Mining (pp. 97–106). New York: Association for Computing Machinery.Google Scholar
Ramos, J. (2003). Using TF-IDF to determine word relevance in document queries. In Proceedings of the First Instructional Conference on Machine Learning Vol.242 (pp. 133–142).Piscataway, NJ: ICML.Google Scholar
Reyes, A., Ross, P., & Veale, T. (2013). A multidimensional approach for detecting irony. Language Resources and Evaluation, 47, 239–268. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, R. M., & Kreuz, R. J. (1994). Why do people use figurative language? Psychological Science, 5, 159–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rockwell, P. (2000). Lower, slower, louder: Vocal cues of sarcasm. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 483–495. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001). Facial expression and sarcasm. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93, 47–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007a). Vocal features of conversational sarcasm: A comparison of methods. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 361–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007b). The effects of cognitive complexity and communication apprehension on the expression and recognition of sarcasm. In A. M. Columbus (Ed.), Advances in psychology research: Volume 49 (pp. 185–196). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Rockwell, P., & Theriot, E. M. (2001). Culture, gender, and gender mix in encoders of sarcasm: A self‐assessment analysis. Communication Research Reports, 18, 44–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schweitzer, J. B., Hanford, R. B., & Medoff, D. R. (2006). Working memory deficits in adults with ADHD: Is there evidence for subtype differences? Behavioral and Brain Functions, 2, 43.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1975). A taxonomy of illocutionary acts. In K. Günderson (Ed.), Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science, volume 7: Language, mind, and knowledge (pp. 344–369). Minneapolis MN: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Shorter Oxford English dictionary, sixth edition. (2007). [CD-ROM version 3.0.2.]. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1981). Irony and the use-mention distinction. In P. Cole (Ed.), Radical pragmatics (pp. 295–318). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tabacaru, S., & Lemmens, M. (2014). Raised eyebrows as gestural triggers in humour: The case of sarcasm and hyper-understanding. The European Journal of Humour Research, 2, 11–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taylor, C. (2015). Beyond sarcasm: The metalanguage and structures of mock politeness. Journal of Pragmatics, 87, 127–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). The relationship between irony and sarcasm: Insights from a first-order metalanguage investigation. Journal of Politeness Research, 2, 209–241.Google Scholar
Thompson, D., & Filik, R. (2016). Sarcasm in written communication: Emoticons are efficient markers of intention. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 21, 105–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, D., Mackenzie, I. G., Leuthold, H., & Filik, R. (2016). Emotional responses to irony and emoticons in written language: Evidence from EDA and facial EMG. Psychophysiology, 53, 1054–1062. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Toplak, M., & Katz, A. N. (2000). On the uses of sarcastic irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 1467–1488. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Utsumi, A. (2000). Verbal irony as implicit display of ironic environment: Distinguishing ironic utterances from nonirony. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 1777–1806. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Voyer, D., & Techentin, C. (2010). Subjective auditory features of sarcasm. Metaphor and Symbol, 25, 227–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wallace, B. C., Choe, D. K., Kertz, L., & Charniak, E. (2014). Humans require context to infer ironic intent (so computers probably do, too). In Proceedings of the 52nd Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (pp. 512–516). Stroudsburg, PA: Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal, and hyperpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23, 3–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Walther, J. B., & D’Addario, K. P. (2001). The impacts of emoticons on message interpretation in computer-mediated communication. Social Science Computer Review, 19, 324–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whalen, J. M., Pexman, P. M., & Gill, A. J. (2009). “Should be fun—not!Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 28, 263–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. A., Burns, E. L., & Harmon, E. A. (2009). Insincere utterances and gaze: Eye contact during sarcastic statements. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 108, 565–572. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, D., & Sperber, D. (1992). On verbal irony. Lingua, 87, 53–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (7)

Cited by seven other publications

Athanasiadou, Angeliki
2023. Constructions in Verbal Irony Production: The Case of Rhetorical Questions. In The Cambridge Handbook of Irony and Thought,  pp. 131 ff. DOI logo
Johnson, Alexander A. & Roger J. Kreuz
2023. Sarcasm Across Time and Space: Patterns of Usage by Age, Gender, and Region in the United States. Discourse Processes 60:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Kałowski, Piotr, Maria Zajączkowska, Katarzyna Branowska, Anna Olechowska, Aleksandra Siemieniuk, Ewa Dryll & Natalia Banasik-Jemielniak
2023. Individual Differences in Verbal Irony Use: A Systematic Review of Quantitative Psycholinguistic Studies. Metaphor and Symbol 38:1  pp. 81 ff. DOI logo
Barnden, John
2021. Metaphor and irony. In Figurative Language – Intersubjectivity and Usage [Figurative Thought and Language, 11],  pp. 139 ff. DOI logo
Barnden, John
2023. Irony, Exaggeration, and Hyperbole: No Embargo on the Cargo!. In The Cambridge Handbook of Irony and Thought,  pp. 272 ff. DOI logo
[no author supplied]
2023. Irony in Linguistic Communication. In The Cambridge Handbook of Irony and Thought,  pp. 129 ff. DOI logo
[no author supplied]
2023. Irony, Affect, and Related Figures. In The Cambridge Handbook of Irony and Thought,  pp. 235 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 19 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.