Abel, E. L., & Kruger, M. L.
(2010) Smile intensity in photographs predicts longevity. Psychological Science, 21(4), 542–544. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Abeles, D., & Yuval-Greenberg, S.
(2017) Just look away: Gaze aversions as an overt attentional disengagement mechanismCognition, 168, 99–109. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adachi, T.
(1996) Sarcasm in Japanese. Studies in Language 20(1), 1–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adams, R. B. Jr. & Kleck, R. E.
(2005) Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotionEmotion, 5(1), 3–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adler, R. M.
(2018) Toward a psycholinguistic model of irony comprehension. [Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland]. [URL].Google Scholar
Adler, R. M., Novick, J. M., & Huang, Y. T.
(2016) The time course of verbal irony comprehension and context integration. In Salfner, F. & Sauerland, E. (Eds.), Pre-proceedings of Trends in Experimental Pragmatics (pp. 1–9). XPRAG.de. [URL].Google Scholar
Amoyal, M., & Priego-Valverde B.
(2019) Smiling for negotiating topic transitions in French conversation. In Grimminger, A. (Ed.), Proceedings of the 6th Gesture and Speech in Interaction – GESPIN 6 (pp. 9–14). University Library Paderborn.Google Scholar
Amoyal, M., Priego-Valverde B., & Rauzy S.
(2020) PACO : A corpus to analyze the impact of common ground in spontaneous face-to-face interaction. Language Resources and Evaluation Conference, LREC 2020, Marseille, France. [URL].Google Scholar
Andersen, P. A.
(2000) Explaining intercultural differences in nonverbal communication. In L. A. Samovar & R. E. Porter (Eds.), Intercultural communication: A reader (9th ed., pp. 258–279). Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Andersen, P. A., Hecht, M., Hoobler, G. D., & Smallwood, M.
(2003) Nonverbal communication across cultures. In W. B. Gudykunst (Ed.), Cross-cultural and intercultural communication (pp. 73–90). SAGE.Google Scholar
Andrist, S., Mutlu, B., & Gleicher, M.
(2013, August) Conversational gaze aversion for virtual agents. In International Workshop on Intelligent Virtual Agents (pp. 249-262). Springer. [URL]. DOI logo
Andrzejewski, S. A., & Mooney, E. C.
(2016) Service with a smile: Does the type of smile matter? Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 29, 135–41.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anolli, L., & Lambiase, L.
(1990) "Giochi di sguardo” nella conversazione. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 17, 27-59.Google Scholar
Anolli, L., Ciceri, R., & Infantino, M. G.
(2000) Irony as a game of implicitness: Acoustic profiles of ironic communication. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 275–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Archakis, A., Giakoumelou, M., Papazachariou, D., & Tsakona, V.
(2010) The prosodic framing of humour in conversational narratives: Evidence from Greek data. Journal of Greek Linguistics, 10(2), 187–212. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Argyle, M.
(1988) [1975]Bodily communication. Routledge.Google Scholar
Argyle, M., & Cook, M.
(1976) Gaze and mutual gaze. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Arizpe, J., Kravitz, D. J., Walsh, V., Yovel, G., & Baker, C. I.
(2016) Differences in looking at own-and other-race faces are subtle and analysis-dependent: An account of discrepant reports. PLoS One, 11(2), e0148253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arslan, A. Ü., Kalkan, S., & Acarturk, C.
(2018) MAGiC: A multimodal framework for analysing gaze in dyadic communication. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 11(6), 1–13.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, D., Churchill, E., Nishino, T., & Okada, H.
(2007) Alignment and interaction in a sociocognitive approach to second language acquisition. The Modern Language Journal, 91(2), 169–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attardo, S.
(1994) Linguistic theories of humor. Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
(2000a) Irony and relevant inappropriateness. Journal of Pragmatics, 32(6), 793–826. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000b) Irony markers and functions: Towards a goal-oriented theory of irony and its processing. Rask, Internationalt Tidsskrift for Sprog og Kommunikation, 12, 3–20.Google Scholar
(2001) Humorous texts: A semantic and pragmatic analysis. De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) Humor and irony in interaction: From mode adoption to failure of detection. In L. Anolli, R. Ciceri, & G. Riva (Eds.), Say not to say: New perspectives on miscommunication (pp. 166–185). IOS Press.Google Scholar
(2015) Encyclopedia of humor studies. SAGE.Google Scholar
(2015) Humor and laughter. In Tannen, D., Hamilton, H. E., & Schiffrin, D. (Eds.), The handbook of discourse analysis (pp. 168–188). Wiley. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019) Humor and mirth. Emotions, embodied cognition, and sustained humor. In J. L. Mackenzie & L. Alba-Juez (Eds.), Emotion in discourse (pp. 189–212). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) The linguistics of humor: An introduction. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021) Humor and cognitive linguistics. In Xu, W., & Taylor, J. R. (Eds.), The Routledge Handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 359–371). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(Ed.) (2017) The Routledge handbook of language and humor. Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attardo, S., & Chabanne, J. C.
(1992) Jokes as a text type. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 5(1–2), 165–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attardo, S., & Pickering, L.
(2011) Timing in the performance of jokes. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 24(2), 233–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attardo, S., & Raskin, V.
(1991) Script theory revis(it)ed: Joke similarity and joke representation model. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 4(3–4), 293–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Linguistics and humor theory. In Attardo, S. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 49–63). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attardo, S., Eisterhold, J., Hay, J., & Poggi, I.
(2003) Multimodal markers of irony and sarcasm. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 16(2), 243–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attardo, S., & Pickering, L.
(forthcoming, 2022) An Introduction to eye-tracking in linguistics: An applied handbook. Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Attardo, S., Pickering, L., & Baker, A.
(2011a) Prosodic and multimodal markers of humor in conversation. Pragmatics and Cognition, 19(2), 224–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attardo, S., Pickering, L., Lomotey, F., & Menjo, S.
(2013) Multimodality in conversational humor. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 11(2), 400–414. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attardo, S., Wagner, M., & Urios-Aparisi, E.
(2011b) Prosody and humor. Pragmatics & Cognition, 19(2), 189–201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aubouin, E.
(1948) Les genres du risible. Ridicule, comique, esprit, humour. OFEP.Google Scholar
Audrieth, A. L.
(1998) The art of using humor in public speaking. [URL] (Part 4. Delivery).Google Scholar
Au-Yeung, S. K., Kaakinen, J. K., Liversedge, S. P., & Benson, V.
(2015) Processing of written irony in Autism Spectrum Disorder: An eye-movement studyAutism Research, 8(6), 749-760. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aviezer, H., Hassin, R., Bentin, S., & Trope, Y.
(2008) Putting facial expressions back in context. In Ambady, N., & Skowronski, J. J. (Eds.), First impressions (pp. 255-286). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Baltrušaitis, T., Robinson, P., & Morency, L.-P.
(2016) OpenFace: An open-source facial behavior analysis toolkit. In The 2016 IEEE Winter Conference on Applications of Computer Vision (WACV) (pp. 1–10). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baltrušaitis, T., Zadeh, A., Lim, Y. C., & Morency, L.-P.
(2018) OpenFace 2.0: Facial Behavior Analysis Toolkit. In The 13th IEEE International Conference on Automatic Face & Gesture Recognition (FG 2018) (pp. 59–66). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bänninger-Hueber, E., & Rauber-Kaiser, S.
(1989) Die Differenzierung verschiedener Lächel-Typen: FACS-Codierung und Einschätzungen. Eine Untersuchung zur Eindrucksbildung. [Differentiation of various types of smiles: FACS-coding and ratings: A study on impression formation.]. Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 48, 21–34.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L.
(1999) The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54(7), 462–479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barisic, I., Timmermans, B., Pfeiffer, U. J., Bente, G., Vogeley, K., & Schilbach, L.
(2013) In it together: Using dual eye tracking to investigate real-time social interactions. In Proceedings from SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. [URL].
Bauman, R.
(1986) Story, performance, and event. Contextual studies of oral narrative. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., & Gerwing, J.
(2011) The listener as addressee in face-to-face dialogueInternational Journal of Listening, 25(3), 178–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Coates, L., & Johnson, T.
(2002) Listener responses as a collaborative process: The role of gazeJournal of Communication, 52(3), 566–580. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bayliss, A. P., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Tipper, S. P.
(2007) Affective evaluations of objects are influenced by observed gaze direction and emotional expressionCognition, 104(3), 644–653. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bayliss, A. P., Paul, M. A., Cannon, P. R., & Tipper, S. P.
(2006) Gaze cuing and affective judgments of objects: I like what you look at. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 1061–1066. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bazzanella, C., & Damiano, R.
(1999) The interactional handling of misunderstanding in everyday conversations. Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 817–836. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beard, F. K.
(2008) Advertising and audience offense: The role of intentional humour. Journal of Marketing Communications, 14 (1), pp. 1–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beattie, G. W.
(1978) Floor apportionment and gaze in conversational dyads. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 17(1), 7-15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beattie, G. W.
(1981) A further investigation of the cognitive interference hypothesis of gaze patterns during conversationBritish Journal of Social Psychology, 20(4), 243–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bell, N.
(2009) Responses to failed humor. Journal of Pragmatics, 41(9), 1825–1836. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009b) Impolite responses to failed humor. In D. Chiaro & N. Norrick (Eds.), Humor in interaction (pp. 143–163). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Responses to incomprehensible humor. Journal of Pragmatics, 57, 176–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) We are not amused: Failed humor in interaction. Mouton De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Failed humor. In Attardo, S. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 356–370). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Pragmatics, humor studies, and the study of interaction. In C. Ilie & N. Norrick (Eds.), Pragmatics and its Interfaces (pp. 291–309). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bell, N., & Attardo, S.
(2010) Failed humor: Issues in non-native speakers’ appreciation and understanding of humor. Intercultural Pragmatics, 7(3), 423–447. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergson, H.
(1901) Laughter: An essay on the meaning of the comic (C. Brereton & F. Rothwell, Trans.). Macmillan. [URL]Google Scholar
Bertrand, R., & Priego-Valverde, B.
(2011) Does prosody play a specific role in conversational humor? Pragmatics and Cognition, 19(2), 333–356. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beukeboom, C. J.
(2009) When words feel right: How affective expressions of listeners change a speaker’s language use. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(5), 747–756. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Birdwhistell, R. L.
(1970) Kinesics and context: Essays on body motion communication. University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Bitterly, T., Brooks, B., & Schweitzer, M. E.
(2017) Risky business: When humor increases and decreases status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(3), 431–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blais, C., Jack, R. E., Scheepers, C., Fiset, D., & Caldara, R.
(2008) Culture shapes how we look at faces. PloS one, 3(8), e3022. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blurton-Jones, N. G.
(1971) Criteria for use in describing facial expressions of children. Human Biology, 43(3), 365–413.Google Scholar
Boxer, D., & Cortés-Conde, F.
(1997) From bonding to biting: Conversational joking and identity display. Journal of Pragmatics, 27(3), 275–294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brône, G.
(2012) Humour and irony in cognitive pragmatics. In Schmid H. (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive pragmatics (pp. 463–504). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Cognitive linguistics and humor research. In Attardo, S. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 250–266). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021) The multimodal negotiation of irony and humor in interaction. On the role of eye gaze in joint pretense. In Soares da Silva, A. (Ed.), Figurative language – Intersubjectivity and usage (pp. 109–136). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brône, G., & Oben, B.
(2015) InSight Interaction: a multimodal and multifocal dialogue corpus. Language Resources and Evaluation, 49(1), 195–214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brône, G., Feyaerts, K., & Veale, T.
(2006) Introduction: Cognitive linguistic approaches to humor. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 19(3), 203–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(Eds.) (2015) Cognitive linguistics and humor research. De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brône, G., Oben, B., Jehoul, A., Vranjes, J., & Feyaerts, K.
(2017) Eye gaze and viewpoint in multimodal interaction management. Cognitive Linguistics, 28(3), 449-483. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Broz, F., Lehmann, H., Nehaniv, C. L., & Dautenhahn, K.
(2012) Mutual gaze, personality, and familiarity: Dual eye-tracking during conversation. In 2012 IEEE RO-MAN: The 21st IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication (pp. 858-864). Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, P., & Levinson, S.
(1987) Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brunner, L. J.
(1979) Smiles can be back channels. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(5), 728–734. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bryant, G. A., & Fox Tree, J. E.
(2002) Recognizing verbal irony in spontaneous speech. Metaphor and Symbol, 17(2), 99–117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Is there an ironic tone of voice? Language and Speech, 48(3), 257–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buján-Navarro, M.
(2019a) Humour production in face-to-face interaction: A multimodal and cognitive study. [Doctoral Dissertation, Universidad de Valladolid]. [URL].Google Scholar
(2019b) The function of face gestures and head movements in spontaneous humorous communication. The European Journal of Humor Research, 7(2). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burgers, C., van Mulken, M., & Schellens, P. J.
(2012) Verbal irony differences in usage across written genresJournal of Language and Social Psychology, 31(3), 290–310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D, B., & Woodall, W. G.
(1989) Nonverbal communication: The unspoken dialogue. HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Burns, L., Marra, M., & Holmes, J.
(2001) Women’s humour in the workplace: A quantitative analysis. Australian Journal of Communication, 28(1), 83–108. Google Scholar
Buswell, G. T.
(1922) Fundamental reading habits: A study of their development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Caldara, R., Zhou, X., & Miellet, S.
(2010) Putting culture under the ‘spotlight’reveals universal information use for face recognition. PLoS One, 5(3), e9708. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Canal, P., Bischetti, L., Di Paola, S., Bertini, C., Ricci, I., & Bambini, V.
(2019) ‘Honey, shall I change the baby? – Well done, choose another one’: ERP and time-frequency correlates of humor processing. Brain and Cognition, 132, 41–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Canestrari, C.
(2010) Meta-communicative signals and humorous verbal interchanges: A case study. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 23(3), 327–349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cappella, J. N.
(1997) Behavioral and judged coordination in adult informal social interactions: Vocal and kinesic indicators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(1), 119–131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. M., & Russell, J. A.
(1996) Do facial expressions signal specific emotions? Judging emotion from the face in context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(2), 205-218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cary, M. S.
(1978) The role of gaze in the initiation of conversation. Social Psychology, 41(3), 269-271. [URL].
Caucci, G. M., & Kreuz, R. J.
(2012) Social and paralinguistic cues to sarcasm. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 25(1), 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W.
(1994) Discourse, consciousness, and time. The flow and displacement of conscious experience in speaking and writing. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Chapman, A. J., & Chapman, W. A.
(1974) Responsiveness to humor: Its dependency upon a companion’s humorous smiling and laughterThe Journal of Psychology, 88(2), 245–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chapman, A. J., & Foot, H. C.
(1977) (Eds.). It’s a funny thing, humor. Proceedings of The International Conference on Humour and Laughter. Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Cheang, H. S., & Pell, M. D.
(2008) The sound of sarcasm. Speech Communication, 50(5), 366–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cheang, H. S., & Pell, M. D.
(2009) Acoustic markers of sarcasm in Cantonese and English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 126(3), 1394-405. . DOI logo. PMID: 19739753.Google Scholar
Cheng, S., Sun, Z., Ma, X., Forlizzi, J. L., Hudson, S. E., & Dey, A.
(2015) Social eye tracking: Gaze recall with online crowds. In Proceedings of the 18th ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work & Social Computing (pp. 454-463). . DOI logo.Google Scholar
Cheyne, J. A.
(1976) Development of forms and functions of smiling in preschoolers. Child Development, 47(3), 820–823. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chiao, J. Y., Harada, T., Komeda, H., Li, Z., Mano, Y., Saito, D., Parrish, T., Sarado N., & Iikada, T.
(2010) Dynamic cultural influences on neural representations of the selfJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(1), 1–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chiaro, D.
(1992) The language of jokes: Analysing verbal play. Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chovanec, J.
(2021) Saving one's face from unintended humour: Impression management in follow-up sports interviews. Journal of Pragmatics, 176, 198-212. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chovil, N.
(1991a) Social determinants of facial displays in conversation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, 141–154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1991b) Discourse-oriented facial displays in conversation. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 25, 163–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christensen, P., Fusaroli, R., & Tylén, K.
(2016) Environmental constraints shaping constituent order in emerging communication systems: Structural iconicity, interactive alignment and conventionalizationCognition, 146, 67–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cienki, A.
(2016) Cognitive linguistics, gesture studies, and multimodal communication. Cognitive Linguistics, 27, 603–618. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clift, R.
(2012) Identifying action: Laughter in non-humorous reported speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 44(10), 1303–1312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clore, G. L.
(1994) Why emotions are felt. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 103-111). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Coates, J.
(2007) Talk in a play frame: More on laughter and intimacy. Journal of Pragmatics, 39, 29–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colston, H. L.
(2017) Irony performance and perception: What underlies verbal, situational and other ironies? In A. Athanasiadou, & H. L. Colston (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 19–42). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) Eye-rolling, irony and embodiment. In A. Athanasiadou, & H. L. Colston (Eds.), The Diversity of irony (pp. 211–235). De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colston, H. L., & Athanasiadou, A.
(2017) Introduction: The irony of irony. In A. Athanasiadou, & H. L. Colston (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 1–16). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Condon, W. S., & Ogston, W. D.
(1966) Sound film analysis of normal and pathological behavior patternsJournal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 143, 338–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Condon, W. S., & Sander, L. W.
(1974) Synchrony demonstrated between movements of the neonate and adult speechChild Development, 45, 456–462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cook, M.
(1977) Gaze and mutual gaze in social encounters: How long – and when – we look others “in the eye” is one of the main signals in nonverbal communicationAmerican Scientist, 65(3), 328–333. [URL].Google Scholar
Cornejo, C., Cuadros, Z., Morales, R., & Paredes, J.
(2017) Interpersonal coordination: Methods, achievements, and challenges. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cornejo, C., Hurtado, E., Cuadros, Z., Torres-Araneda, A., Paredes, J., Olivares, H., Carré, D., & Robledo, J. P.
(2018) Dynamics of simultaneous and imitative bodily coordination in trust and distrust. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cornew, L., Carver, L., & Love, T.
(2010) There’s more to emotion than meets the eye: A processing bias for neutral content in the domain of emotional prosody. Cognition and Emotion, 24(7), 1133–1152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coulson, S., & Kutas, M.
(1998) Frame-shifting and sentential integrationCognitive Science Technical Report, 98(02).Google Scholar
(2001) Getting it: Human event-related brain response to jokes in good and poor comprehenders. Neuroscience Letters, 316(2), 71–4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coulson, S., & Lowett, C.
(2004) Handedness, hemispheric asymmetries, and joke comprehension. Cognitive Brain Research, 19, 275–288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coulson, S., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M.
(2006) Looking back: Joke comprehension and the space structuring model. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 19(3), 229–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crafa, D., Liu, J. Q., & Brodeur, M. B.
(2019) Social values and determinants of cultural fit in Quebec: The roles of ancestry, linguistic group, and mental health statusFrontiers in Psychology, 10.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crafa, D., Schiff, J., & Brodeur, M. B.
(2019) Social interaction alters self-identity: Adapted methods for measuring active self-concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, in review. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Creswell, J. W., & Plano Clark, V.
(2011) Designing and conducting mixed methods research. Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Crivelli, C., Carrera, P., & Fernández-Dols, J. M.
(2015) Are smiles a sign of happiness? Spontaneous expressions of judo winnersEvolution and Human Behavior, 36, 52–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crystal, D., & Davy, D.
(1969) Investigating English style. Longman.Google Scholar
Cummins, F.
(2012) Gaze and blinking in dyadic conversation: A study in coordinated behaviour among individuals. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(10), 1525-1549. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A.
(1974). On saying what you mean without meaning what you say. In M. Galy, R. Fox, & A. Bruck (Eds.), Papers from the Tenth Regional Meeting, Chicago Linguistic Society (pp. 117–127). Chicago Linguistic Society. [URL].Google Scholar
Dale, R., Bryant, G. A., Manson, J. H., & Gervais, M. M.
(2020) Body synchrony in triadic interaction. Royal Society Open Science, 7 (200095). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dale, R., Fusaroli, R., Håkonsson, D., Healey, P., Mønster, D., McGraw, J., Mitkidis, P., Tylén, K.
(2013) Beyond synchrony: Complementarity and asynchrony in joint action, Proceedings of CognSci 2013, 35, 79–80.Google Scholar
Davies, Christie
. (1990) Ethnic humor around the world: A comparative analysis. Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
. (1998) Jokes and their relationship to society. Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Davies, Christine
. (1984) Joint joking: Improvisational humorous episodes in conversation. Proceedings of the Tenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 360–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davitti, E., & Pasquandrea, S.
(2017) Embodied participation: What multimodal analysis can tell us about interpreter-mediated encounters in pedagogical settings. Journal of Pragmatics, 107, 105–128. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Jongste, H.
(2013) Negotiating humorous intent. In Dynel, M. (Ed.), Developments in linguistic humour theory (pp. 179-210). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Delabarre, E. B.
(1898) A method of recording eye-movements. The American Journal of Psychology, 9(4), 572–574. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dimberg, U., & Petterson, M.
(2000) Facial reactions to happy and angry facial expressions: evidence for right hemisphere dominance. Psychophysiology, 37(5), 693–696. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K.
(2000) Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 11(1), 86–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dodd, D. K., Russell B. L., & Jenkins C.
(1999) Smiling in school yearbook photos: Gender differences from kindergarten to adulthood. Psychological Research, 49, 543–553. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Doherty-Sneddon, G., Bonner, L., & Bruce, V.
(2001) Cognitive demands of face monitoring: Evidence for visuospatial overloadMemory & Cognition, 29(7), 909–919. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Doherty-Sneddon, G., Bruce, V., Bonner, L., Longbotham, S., & Doyle, C.
(2002) Development of gaze aversion as disengagement from visual informationDevelopmental Psychology, 38(3), 438–445.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Doherty-Sneddon, G., & Phelps, F. G.
(2005) Gaze aversion: A response to cognitive or social difficulty? Memory & cognition, 33(4), 727–733. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dore, M.
(2018) Controversial humour in advertising: Social and cultural implications. In Maon, F., Lindgreen, A., Vanhamme, J., Angell, R. J., & Memery, J. (Eds.), Not all claps and cheers: Humour in business and society relationships (pp. 132–145). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) Intertextuality and failed taboo humour in advertising. European Journal of Humor Research, 8(3) 99–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drahota, A., Costall, A., & Reddy, V.
(2008) The vocal communication of different kinds of smile. Speech Communication, 50(4), 278–287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drew, P.
(1987) Po-faced receipts of teases. Linguistics, 25(1), 219–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duchowski, A. T.
(2017) Eye tracking methodology: Theory and practice. Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duncan, S., Brunner, L. J., & Fiske, D. W.
(1979) Strategy signals in face-to-face interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(2), 301–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duncan, S., & Fiske, D. W.
(2015). Face-to-face interaction: Research, methods, and theory. (2nd ed.) Routledge. (Original work published 1977). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dynel, M.
(2009) Beyond a joke: Types of conversational humour. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(5), 1284–1299. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Academics vs. American scriptwriters vs. academics: A battle over the etic and emic “sarcasm” and “irony” labels. Language and Communication, 55, 69–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) No child’s play: A philosophical pragmatic view of overt pretence as a vehicle for conversational humour. In Tsakona, V. & Chovanec, J. (Eds), The dynamics of interactional humour: Creating and negotiating humour in everyday encounters (pp. 205–228). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ehrlichman, H.
(1981) From gaze aversion to eye-movement suppression: An investigation of the cognitive interference explanation of gaze patterns during conversationBritish Journal of Social Psychology, 20(4), 233–241.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Einav, S., & Hood, B. M.
(2008) Tell-tale eyes: children’s attribution of gaze aversion as a lying cueDevelopmental Psychology, 44(6), 1655–1667. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eisterhold, J.
(2007) Failed humor in American discourse. Proceedings of the Paper Presented at International Society for Humor Studies, Newport.Google Scholar
Eisterhold, J., Attardo, S., Boxer, D.
(2006) Reactions to irony in discourse: Evidence for the least disruption principle. Journal of Pragmatics, 38(8), 1239–1256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ekman P.
(Ed.) (1973) Darwin and facial expression: A century of research in review. Malor Books.Google Scholar
Ekman, P.
(2007). The directed facial action task: Emotional responses without appraisal. In J. A. Coan & J. J. B. Allen (Eds.), Series in affective science. Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment (pp. 47–53). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V.
(1969) Nonverbal leakage and clues to deception. Psychiatry, 32, 88–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1978) Facial action coding system. Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
(1982) Felt, false, and miserable smiles. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 6(4), 238–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., Davidson, R. J., & Friesen, W. V.
(1990) The Duchenne smile: Emotional expression and brain physiology II. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 342–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Hager, J. C.
(Eds.) (2002) Facial action coding system. Research Nexus.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Rolls, E. T., Perrett, D. I., & Ellis, H. D.
(1992) Facial expressions of emotion: An old controversy and new findings [and discussion]. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 335(1273), 63–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V.
(1969) Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotionScience, 164(3875), 86–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N.
(2002) On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysisPsychological Bulletin, 128(2), 203–235.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
El Haddad, K., Chakravarthula, S. N., & Kennedy, J.
Smile and laugh dynamics in naturalistic dyadic interactions: Intensity levels, sequences and roles. In Gao, W. et al. Eds. Proceedings of the 2019 International Conference on Multimodal Interaction pp. 259 263 Association for Computing Machinery. DOI logo
Emery, N. J.
(2000) The eyes have it: The neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(6), 581–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Erickson, F.
(2004) Origins: A brief intellectual and technological history of the emergence of multimodal discourse analysis. In P. LeVine & R. Scollon (Eds.), Discourse and technology. Multimodal discourse analysis (pp. 196–207). Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Ervas, F.
(2020) How nice does it sound? An argumentative approach to the affective aspects of irony production. In Barnden, J., & Gargett, A. (Eds.), Producing figurative expression: Theoretical, experimental and practical perspectives (pp. 175–210). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Everts, E.
(2003) Identifying a particular family humor style: A sociolinguistic discourse analysis. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 16(4), 369–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fagel, S.
(2010) Effects of smiling on articulation: Lips, larynx and acoustics. In A. Esposito, A. Campbell, N., Vogel C., Hussain A., & Nijholt A. (Eds.), COST 2102 Int. Training School 2009, LNCS 5967 (pp. 294–303). Springer-Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernández-Dols, J. M., & Ruiz-Belda, M. A.
(1995) Are smiles a sign of happiness? Gold medal winners at the Olympic GamesJournal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1113–1119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferstl, E., Israel, L. & Putzar, L.
(2016) Humor facilitates text comprehension: Evidence from eye movements. Discourse Processes, 54(4), 259–284. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
File, K. A., & Schnurr, S.
(2019) That match was “a bit like losing your virginity”. Failed humour, face and identity construction in TV interviews with professional athletes and coaches. Journal of Pragmatics 152, 132–144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Filik, R., & Moxey, L. M.
(2010) The on-line processing of written irony. Cognition, 116, 421–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Filik, R., Leuthold, H., Wallington, K., & Page, J.
(2014) Testing theories of irony processing using eye tracking and ERPs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 811–828.Google Scholar
Filik, R., Brightman, E., Gathercole, C., & Leuthold, H.
(2017) The emotional impact of verbal irony: Eye tracking evidence for a two-stage process. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 193–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Filik, R., Howman, H., Ralph-Nearman, C., & Giora, R.
(2018) The role of defaultness and personality factors in sarcasm interpretation: Evidence from eye tracking during reading. Metaphor and Symbol, 33(3), 148–162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fischer, A. H.
(2001) Gender and emotion social psychological perspectives. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Flamson, T., Bryant, G. A., & Barrett, H. C.
(2011) Prosody in spontaneous humor. Evidence for encryption. Pragmatics & Cognition, 19(2), 248–267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fónagy, I.
(1971) Synthèse de l’ironie. Phonetica, 23, 42–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frank, M. G., & Ekman, P.
(1993) Not all smiles are created equal: The differences between enjoyment and nonenjoyment smilesHUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 6(1), 9–26.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Freese, J., Meland, S., & Irwin, W.
(2006) Expressions of positive emotion in photographs, personality, and later-life marital and health outcomes. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(2), 488–497. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Freud, S.
(1916) Wit and its relation to the unconscious (A. Brill, Trans). Moffat, Yard, and Company. [URL]Google Scholar
Fridlund, A. J.
(1991) Sociality of solitary smiling: potentiation by an implicit audience. Journal of personality and social psychology, 60(2), 229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994) Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. San Diego, CA: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
(2017) The behavioral ecology view of facial displays, 25 years later. In J.-M. Fernández-Dols & J. A. Russell (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 77–92). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fridlund, A. J., Kenworthy, K. G., & Jaffey, A. K.
(1992) Audience effects in affective imagery: Replication and extension to dysphoric imagery. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 16(3), 191-212. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fu, G., Hu, C. S., Wang, Q., Quinn, P. C., & Lee, K.
(2012) Adults scan own-and other-race faces differently. PloS one, 7(6), e37688. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fujiwara, K., Kimura, M., & Daibo, I.
(2019) Rhythmic features of movement synchrony for bonding individuals in dyadic interaction. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 44, 173–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Frith, C. D., Roepstorff, A., & Tylén, K.
(2012) Coming to terms: An experimental quantification of the coordinative benefits of linguistic interaction. Psychological Science, 23(8), 931–939. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., & Tylén, K.
(2014) Dialog as interpersonal synergyNew Ideas in Psychology, 32, 147–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fydanaki, A., & Geradts, Z.
(2018) Evaluating OpenFace: an open-source automatic facial comparison algorithm for forensicsForensic Sciences Research, 3(3), 202–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J.
(2009) Joint action, interactive alignment, and dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 292–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
George, N., & Conty, L.
(2008) Facing the gaze of othersNeurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology, 38(3), 197–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W.
(1999) Intentions in the experience of meaning. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. Jr. Bryant, G. A., & Colston, H. L.
(2014) Where is the humor in verbal irony? HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 27(4), 575–595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giora, R.
(1991) On the cognitive aspects of the joke. Journal of Pragmatics, 16(5), 465–485. . DOI logo.Google Scholar
(1997) Understanding figurative and literal language: The graded salience hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics, 7, 182–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003) On our mind: Salience, context, and figurative language. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giora, R., Fein, O., Kronrod, A., Elnatan, I., Shuval, N., & Zur, A.
(2004) Weapons of mass distraction: Optimal Innovation and pleasure ratings. Metaphor and Symbol, 19, 115–141. . DOI logo.Google Scholar
Giora, R., Givoni, S., & Fein, O.
(2015) Defaultness reigns: The case of sarcasm. Metaphor and symbol, 30(4), 290-313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Girard, J. M., Cohn, J. F., Yin, L., & Morency, L.-P.
(2021) Reconsidering the Duchenne smile: Formalizing and testing hypotheses about eye constriction and positive emotion. Affective Science. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Girard, J. M., Shandar, G., Liu, Z., Cohn, J. F., Yin, L., & Morency, L.-P.
(2019) Reconsidering the Duchenne smile: indicator of positive emotion or artifact of smile intensity? Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction (ACII), 594–599. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gironzetti, E.
(2017a) Multimodal and eye-tracking evidence in the negotiation of pragmatic intentions in dyadic conversations: The case of humorous discourse [Doctoral dissertation, Texas A&M University-Commerce].Google Scholar
(2017b) Prosodic and multimodal markers of humor. In S. Attardo (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 400–413). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) Eye-tracking applications for Spanish pragmatics research. In Koike, D. and Felix-Brasdefer, C. (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of Spanish pragmatics: Foundations and interfaces (pp. 517–531). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021a) Multimodal resources in the co-construction of humorous discourse. In L. Czerwionka, R. Showstack, & J. Liskin-Gasparro (Eds.), Contexts of discourse: Co-construction, pragmatics, and curricular approaches (pp. 115–135). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gironzetti, E., Attardo, S., & Pickering, L.
(2016a) Smiling, gaze, and humor in conversation. A pilot study. In Ruíz Gurillo, L. (Ed.), Metapragmatics of humor: Current research trends (pp. 235–254). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Smiling and the negotiation of humor in conversationDiscourse Processes, 56(7), 496–512. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gironzetti, E., Pickering, L., Huang, M., Zhang, Y., Menjo, S., & Attardo, S.
(2016b) Smiling synchronicity and gaze patterns in dyadic humorous conversations. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 29(2), 301–324. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M., Schroeder, J. L., & Robertson, D. A.
(1998) Averting the gaze disengages the environment and facilitates rememberingMemory & cognition, 26(4), 651–658. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glenn, P., & Holt, E.
(2013) Studies of laughter in interaction. Bloomsbury. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goddard, C., & Mullan, K.
(2019) Explicating verbs for “laughing with other people” in French and English (and why it matters for humour studies). HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 33(1), 55–77. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Godfroid, A.
(2019) Eye tracking in second language acquisition and bilingualism: A research synthesis and methodological guide. Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goffman, E.
(1967) Interactional ritual: Essays in face-to-face behavior. Doubleday.Google Scholar
(1974) Frame analysis: An essay on the organization of experience. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(1981) Forms of talk. University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S.
 (2005). Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think.  Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, J. H., & McGhee, P. E.
(Eds.) (1972) The psychology of humor: Theoretical perspectives and empirical issues. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Golland, Y., Arzouan, Y., & Levit-Binnun, N.
(2015) The mere co-presence: Synchronization of autonomic signals and emotional responses across co-present individuals not engaged in direct interactionPLOS ONE, 10(5), e0125804. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gonzaga, G. C., Turner, R. A., Keltner, D., Campos, B., & Altemus, M.
(2006) Romantic love and sexual desire in close relationships. Emotion, 6(2), 163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
González-Fuente, S., Escandell-Vidal, V., & Prieto, P.
(2015) Gestural codas pave the way to the understanding of verbal ironyJournal of Pragmatics, 90, 26–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grant, E. C.
(1969) Human facial expression. Man, 4, 525–536. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, M.
(2019) Organic meaning: An approach to communication with minimal appeal to minds. In Capone A., Carapezza M., Lo Piparo F. (Eds), Further advances in pragmatics and philosophy: Theories and Applications: Part 2 Theories and Applications. Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology, vol 20. Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Précis of self-expression (Oxford, 2007). Acta Analitica, 25, 65–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gregory, S. W., & Webster, S.
(1996) A nonverbal signal in voices of interview partners effectively predicts communication accommodation and social status perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6), 1231–1240. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P.
(1989) Studies in the way of words. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Guastello, S. J., Pincus, D., & Gunderson, P. R.
(2006) Electrodermal arousal between participants in a conversation: nonlinear dynamics and linkage effectsNonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences, 10(3), 365–399.Google Scholar
Gulas, C. S., & Weinberger, M. G.
(2006) Humour in advertising: A comprehensive analysis. M. E. Sharpe.Google Scholar
Gullberg, M., & Kita, S.
(2009) Attention to speech-accompanying gestures: Eye movements and information uptake. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33(4), 251–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gumperz, J. J.
(1964) Linguistic and social interaction in two communities. American Anthropologist, 66(6), 137–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gunnery, S. D., & Hall, J. A.
(2014) The Duchenne smile and persuasionJournal of Nonverbal Behavior, 38(2), 181–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gunnery, S. D., & Ruben, M. A.
(2016) Perceptions of Duchenne and non-Duchenne smiles: A meta-analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 30(3), 501–515. . DOI logo.Google Scholar
Gunnery, S. D., Hall, J. A., & Ruben, M. A.
(2013) The deliberate Duchenne smile: Individual differences in expressive controlJournal of Nonverbal Behavior, 37, 29–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Günther, U. K.
(2003) What’s in a laugh? Humour, jokes and laughter in the conversational corpus of the BNC [Doctoral dissertation, University of Freiburg].Google Scholar
Haakana, M.
(2010). Laughter and smiling: Notes on co-occurrencesJournal of Pragmatics, 41, 1499–1512.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haensel, J. X., Danvers, M., Ishikawa, M., Itakura, S. Tucciarelli, R., Smith, T. J., & Senju, A.
(2020). Culture modulates face scanning during dyadic social interactionsNature Scientific Reports, 10, 1958. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haiman, J.
(1998) Talk is cheap: Sarcasm, alienation, and the evolution of language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hall, J. A.
(1984) Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
(2006). Women’s and men’s nonverbal communication: Similarities, differences, stereotypes, and origins. In V. Manusov & M. L. Patterson (Eds.), The Sage handbook of nonverbal communication  (pp. 201–218). Sage Publications. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hall, J. A., & Halberstadt, A. G.
(1986) Smiling and gazing. In J. S. Hyde & M. C. Inn (Eds.), The psychology of gender: Advances through meta-analysis (pp. 136–185). Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Hansen, Z., Niedenthal, P., Martin, J., & Wood, A.
(2020) Gender differences in the form and function of naturally occurring smiles. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harker, L., & Keltner, D.
(2001) Expression of positive emotion in women’s college yearbook pictures and their relationship to personality and life outcomes across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 112–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, C. R., & Alvarado, N.
(2005) Facial expressions, smile types, and self-report during humour, tickle, and pain. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 655–669. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hasson, U., Ghazanfar, A. A., Galantucci, B., Garrod, S., & Keysers, C.
(2012) Brain-to-brain coupling: a mechanism for creating and sharing a social worldTrends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 114–121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hatasa, K.
(2012) Integrating “Rakugo” and “Kobanashi” in Japanese language classes at different levels. Japanese Language and Literature, 46(1), 201–215. [URL].Google Scholar
Hatzithomas, L., Boutsouki, C. & Zotos, Y.
(2016) The role of economic conditions on humour generation and attitude towards humorous TV commercials. HUMOR – International Journal of Humour Research, 29(4), pp. 483–505. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haugh, M., & Bousfield, D.
(2012) Mock impoliteness, jocular mockery and jocular abuse in Australian and British English. Journal of Pragmatics, 44(9), 1099–1114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haugh, M.
(2008) The place of intention in the interactional achievement of implicature. In I. Kecskes, & J. Mey (Eds.), Intention, common ground and the egocentric speaker-hearer (pp. 45–86). Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Haviland, J. M.
(1977) Sex-related pragmatics in infants’ nonverbal communication. Journal of Communication, 27(2), 80–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hay, J.
(1994a) Humour support strategies and failed humour. In Proceedings of the First Annual New Zealand Post-Graduate Student Conference: Speaking for Ourselves. Victoria, University of Wellington, December 1994.Google Scholar
(1994b) Jocular abuse patterns in mixed-group interaction. Wellington Working Papers in Linguistics, 6, 26–55.Google Scholar
(1995) Gender and humour: Beyond a joke [Master’s thesis, Victoria University of Wellington].Google Scholar
(2001) The pragmatics of humor support. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 14(1), 55–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hayward, D. A., Voorhies, W., Morris, J. L., Capozzi, F., & Ristic, J.
(2017) Staring reality in the face: A comparison of social attention across laboratory and real-world measures suggests little common ground. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 71(3), 212–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heerey, E. A., & Crossley, H. M.
(2013) Predictive and reactive mechanisms in smile reciprocity. Psychological Science, 24(8), 1446–1455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heerey, E. A., & Kring, A. M.
(2007) Interpersonal consequences of social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 125–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hering, E.
(1879) Über Muskelgeräusche des Auges. [‘On muscle sounds of the eye’]. Sitzungsberichte der kaiserlichen Akademie der Wissenschaften in Wien. Mathematisch-naturwissenschaftliche Klasse, 3(79), 137–154.Google Scholar
Hertenstein, M. J., Hansel, C. A., Butts A. M., & Hile S. N.
(2009) Smile intensity in photographs predicts divorce later in life. Motivation and Emotion, 33(2), 99–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., & Blairy, S.
(2001) Facial mimicry and emotional contagion to dynamic emotional facial expressions and their influence on decoding accuracy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40(2), 129–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., & Bourgeois, P.
(2010) You smile–I smile: Emotion expression in social interaction. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 514–520. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., Banse, R., & Kappas, A.
(1995) The intensity of facial expression is determined by underlying affective state and social situation. Journal of personality and social psychology, 69(2), 280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., Senécal, S., Kirouac, G., Herrera, P., Philippot, P., & Kleck, R. E.
(2000) Emotional expressivity in men and women: Stereotypes and self-perceptions. Cognition & Emotion, 14(5), 609-642. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B., Grammer, K., & Kleck, R. E.
(2009) Face gender and emotion expression: Are angry women more like men?. Journal of Vision, 9(12), 19-19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hessels, R. S.
(2020) How does gaze to faces support face-to-face interaction? A review and perspectivePsychonomic Bulletin Review, 27, 856–881. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hessels, R. S., Niehorster, D. C., Nyström, M., Andersson, R., & Hooge, I. T. C.
(2018) Is the eye-movement field confused about fixations and saccades? A survey among 124 researchersRoyal Society Open Science, 5, 180502. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hills, P. J., Ross, D. A., & Lewis, M. B.
(2011) Attention misplaced: The role of diagnostic features in the face-inversion effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), 1396–1406. [URL].Google Scholar
Hinsz, V. B., & Tomhave, J. A.
(1991) Smile and (half ) the world smiles with you, frown and you frown alone. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 586–92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ho, S., Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A.
(2015) Speaking and listening with the eyes: gaze signaling during dyadic interactionsPloS one, 10(8), e0136905. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hockett, C. F.
(1977) Jokes. In Hockett, C. F. (Ed), The view from language, Selected essays. University of Georgia Press.Google Scholar
Holler, J., & Bavelas, J.
(2017). Multi-modal communication of common ground: A review of social functions. In R. B. Church, M. W. Alibali, & S. D. Kelly (Eds.), Why gesture? How the hands function in speaking, thinking and communicating (pp. 213–240). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holler, J., Kendrick, K. H. & Levinson, S. C.
(2018) Processing language in face-to-face conversation: Questions with gestures get faster responses. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 1900–1908. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holmes, J., & Hay, J.
(1997) Humour as an ethnic boundary marker in New Zealand interaction. Journal of Intercultural Studies, 18(2), 127–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holmqvist, K., Nyström, M., Andersson, R., Dewhurst, R., Jarodzka, H., & Van de Weijer, J.
(2011). Eye tracking: A comprehensive guide to methods and measures. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L.
(2009) It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27, 949–960. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsiao, J. H.-W., & Cottrell, G.
(2008) Two fixations suffice in face recognition. Psychological Science, 19(10), 998–1006. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsu, H. C., Brône, G., & Feyaerts, K.
(2020) When gesture “takes over”: Speech-embedded nonverbal depictions in multimodal interactionFrontiers in Psychology, 11 3169 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hu, C., Wang, Q., Fu, G., Quinn, P. C., & Lee, K.
(2014) Both children and adults scan faces of own and other races differently. Vision research, 102, 1-10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huey, E. B.
(1900) On the psychology and physiology of reading. The American Journal of Psychology, 11(3), 283–302. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1989) Preliminary experiments in the physiology and psychology of reading. The American Journal of Psychology, 9(4), 575–586. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hymes, D.
(1972) Models of the Interaction of Language. Language, Culture and Society, 189-223.Google Scholar
Ikeda, K., & Bysouth, D.
(2013) Laughter and turn-taking: Warranting next speakership in multiparty interactions. In P. J. Glenn & E. Holt (Eds.), Studies of laughter in interaction (pp. 39–64). Bloomsbury Academic. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E.
(1997). Emotions and facial expressions: A perspective from differential emotions theory. In J. A. Russell & J. M. Fernández-Dols (Eds.), Studies in emotion and social interaction, 2nd series. The psychology of facial expression (p. 57–77). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jack, R. E., Blais, C., Scheepers, C., Schyns, P. G., & Caldara, R.
(2009) Cultural confusions show that facial expressions are not universal. Current Biology, 19(18), 1543-1548. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jakonen, T., & Evnitskaya, N.
(2020) Teacher smiles as an interactional and pedagogical resource in the classroom. Journal of Pragmatics, 163, 18–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jansen, N., & Chen, A.
(2020) Prosodic encoding of sarcasm at the sentence level in Dutch. In N. Minematsu (Ed.), Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Speech Prosody 2020 (pp. 409–413). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Javal, L. É.
(1879) Essai sur la physiologie de la lecture. Annales d’Oculistique, 82, 242–253.Google Scholar
Jefferson, G.
(1979) A technique for inviting laughter and its subsequent acceptance declination. In G. Psathas (Ed.), Everyday language: Studies in ethnomethodology (pp. 79–96). Irvington Publishers.Google Scholar
(1984) On the organization of laughter in talk about trouble. In Atkinson, J. M., & Heritage, J. C.  (Eds.), Structures of social action: Studies in conversation analysis (pp. 346–369). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jefferson, G., Sacks, H., & Schegloff, E.
(1987) Notes on laughter in the pursuit of intimacy. In G. Button & J. R. E. Lee (Eds.), Talk and social organisation (pp. 152–205). Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
(1978) Notes on laughter in the pursuit of intimacy. In G. Button & J. R. E. Lee (Eds.), Talk and Social Organisation (pp. 152–205). Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Jefferson, G., Sacks, H., & Schegloff, E. A.
(1977) Preliminary notes on the sequential organization of laughterPragmatics Microfiche. Cambridge University Department of Linguistics. [URL].Google Scholar
Jehoul, A., Brône, G., & Feyaerts, K.
(2017) The shrug as marker of obviousness: Corpus evidence from Dutch face-to-face conversationsLinguistics Vanguard, 3(s1), 20160082. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jensen, M.
(2015) Smile as feedback expressions in interpersonal interaction. International Journal of Psychological Studies, 7(4), 95–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jokinen K.
(2010) Pointing gestures and synchronous communication management. In Esposito A., Campbell, N., Vogel C., Hussain A., Nijholt A. (Eds), Development of multimodal interfaces: Active listening and synchrony (pp. 33–49). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jokinen, K., Furukawa, H., Nishida, M., & Yamamoto, S.
(2013) Gaze and turn-taking behavior in casual conversational interactions. ACM Transactions on Interactive Intelligent Systems (TiiS), 3(2), 1-30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jones, S. S., Collins, K., & Hong, H. W.
(1991) An audience effect on smile production in 10-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 2(1), 45-49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaakinen, J. K., Olkoniemi, H., Kinnari, T., & Hyönä, J.
(2014) Processing of written irony: An eye movement study. Discourse Processes, 51(4), 287-311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaczmarek, L. D., Behnke, M., Kashdan, T. B., Kusiak, A., Marzec, K., Mistrzak, M., & Włodarczyk, M.
(2018) Smile intensity in social networking profile photographs is related to greater scientific achievements. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 13(5), 435-439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kadooka, K.
(2012) An acoustic analysis of the punch line paratone in the Japanese Kobanashi stories. The Ryukoku Journal of Humanities and Sciences, 33(2), 17–37.Google Scholar
Kang, O., Rubin, D., & Pickering, L.
(2010) Suprasegmental measures of accentedness and judgments of language learner proficiency in oral English. Modern Language Journal, 94(4), 554–566. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaukomaa, T., Peräkylä, A., & Ruusuvuori, J.
(2013) Turn-opening smiles: Facial expression constructing emotional transition in conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 55, 21–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kayyal, M., Widen, S., & Russell, J. A.
(2015) Context is more powerful than we think: Contextual cues override facial cues even for valence. Emotion, 15(3), 287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, T., Iwatate, T., & Takanashi, K.
(2012) Prediction of turn-taking by combining prosodic and eye-gaze information in poster conversations. In Thirteenth Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association. [URL]. DOI logo
Keltner, D.
(1995) Signs of appeasement: Evidence for the distinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and shameJournal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(3), 441–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keltner, D., & Cordaro, D. T.
(2017) Understanding multimodal emotional expressions: Recent advances in basic emotion theory. In J.-M. Fernández-Dols & J. A. Russell (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 57–76). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M.
(1998) Emotion, social function, and psychopathology. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 320-342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keltner, D., & Lerner, J. S.
(2010) Emotion. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 317–352). John Wiley & Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A.
(1967) Some functions of gaze-direction in social interaction. Acta Psychologica, 26(1), 22–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1970) Movement coordination in social interaction: some example describedActa Psychologica, 32, 100–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendrick, K. H., & Holler, J.
(2017) Gaze direction signals response preference in conversationResearch on Language and Social Interaction, 50(1), 12–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kesey, K.
(1962) One flew over the cuckoo’s nest. Signet.Google Scholar
Kingstone, A.
(2009) Taking a real look at social attention. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19, 52–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kleinke, C. L.
(1986) Gaze and eye contact: A research reviewPsychological bulletin, 100(1), 78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knackstedt, G., & Kleinke, C. L.
(1991) Eye contact, gender, and personality judgments. The Journal of social psychology, 131(2), 303-304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koban, L., Ramamoorthy, A., & Konvalinka, I.
(2019) Why do we fall into sync with others? Interpersonal synchronization and the brain’s optimization principle. Social Neuroscience, 14(1) 1–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koike, D., & Czerwionka, L.
(2016) Diálogo. In J. Gutiérrez-Rexach (Ed.), Enciclopedia lingüística hispánica, Vol. 2 (pp. 405–412). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koike, D.
(2012) Variation in NS-learner interactions. Frames and expectations in pragmatic co-construction. In C. Felix-Brasdefer & D. Koike (Eds.), Pragmatic variation in first and second language contexts: Methodological issues (pp. 175–208). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kok, K. I., & Cienki, A.
(2016) Cognitive grammar and gesture: Points of convergence, advances and challenges. Cognitive Linguistics, 27(1), 67–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kotthoff, H.
(2000) Gender and joking: On the complexities of women’s image politics in humorous narratives. Journal of Pragmatics, 32(1), 55–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003) Responding to irony in different contexts: On cognition and conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 35, 1387–1411. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Joint construction of humorous fictions in conversation. An unnamed narrative activity in a playful keying. Journal of Literary Theory, 3(2), 195–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koutsombogera, M., & Papageorgiou, H.
(2010) Linguistic and non-verbal cues for the induction of silent feedback. In Esposito A., Campbell, N., Vogel C., Hussain A., Nijholt A. (Eds), Development of multimodal interfaces: Active listening and synchrony (pp. 327–336). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kraut, R. E., & Johnston, R. E.
(1979) Social and emotional messages of smiling: an ethological approach. Journal of personality and social psychology, 37(9), 1539. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kreuz, R. J.
(1996) The use of verbal irony: Cues and constraints. In Mio, J. S., & Katz, A. N. (Eds.). Metaphor: Implications and applications (pp. 23–38). Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., & Glucksberg, S.
(1989) How to be sarcastic: The echoic reminder theory of verbal irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118(4), 374–386. . DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., & Roberts, R. M.
(1995) Two cues to verbal irony: Hyperbole and the ironic tone of voice. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10(1), 21–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., Kassler, M. A., Coppenrath, L., & Allen, B. M.
(1999) Tag questions and common ground effects in the perception of verbal ironyJournal of Pragmatics, 31(12), 1685–1700. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kreuz, R., & Caucci, G.
(2007) Lexical influences in the perception of sarcasm. In A. Feldman & X Lu (Eds), Proceedings of the Workshop on Computational Approaches to Figurative Language (pp. 1–4). Association for Computational Linguistics. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krumhuber, E. G., & Manstead, A. S. R.
(2009) Can Duchenne smiles be feigned? New evidence on felt and false smilesEmotion, 9(6), 807–820. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krys, K., Vauclair, C. M., Capaldi, C. A., Miu-Chi Lun, V., Harris Bond, M., Domínguez-Espinosa, A., Torres, C., Lipp, O. V., Manickam, L. S. S., Xing, C., Antalíková, R., Pavlopoulos, V., Teyssier, J., Hur, T., Hansen, K., Szarota, P., Ahmed, R. A., Burtceva, E., Chkhaidze, A., … Arriola Yu, A.
(2016) Be careful where you smile: Culture shapes judgments of intelligence and honesty of smiling individualsJournal of Nonverbal Behavior, 40, 101–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuipers, G.
(2011/2015) Good humor, bad taste: A sociology of the joke. Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kunz, M., Prkachin, K., & Lautenbacher, S.
(2009) The smile of painPain, 145(3), 273–275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
LaFrance, M., Hecht, M. A., & Paluck, E. L.
(2003) The contingent smile: a meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. Psychological bulletin, 129(2), 305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laineste, L.
(2013) Funny or aggressive? Failed humour in internet comments. Folklore: Electronic Journal of Folklore, 53, 29–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L.
(2003) Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14(4), 334–339. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lamare, M.
(1892) Des mouvements des yeux dans la lecture. Bulletins et Mémoires de la Société Française d’Ophtalmologie, 10, 354–364.Google Scholar
Larkin-Galiñanes, C.
(2017) An overview of humor theory. In Attardo, S. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 4–16). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laval, V., & Bert-Erboul, A.
(2005) French-speaking children’s understanding of sarcasm: The role of intonation and context. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 610–620. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leggitt, J. S., & Gibbs, R. W.
(2000) Emotional reactions to verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 29, 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lehtimaja, I.
(2011) Teacher-oriented address terms in students’ reproach turns. Linguistics and Education, 22(4), 348–363. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levine, M. H., & Sutton-Smith, B.
(1973) Effects of age, sex, and task on visual behavior during dyadic interaction. Developmental Psychology, 9(3), 400. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Louwerse, M. M., Dale, R., Bard, E. G., & Jeuniaux, P.
(2012) Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronizedCognitive Science, 36(8), 1404–1426. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lumsden, J., Miles, L. K., Richardson, M. J., Smith, C. A., & Macrae, C. N.
(2012) Who syncs? Social motives and interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(3), 746–751. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lynn, J. G.
(1940) An apparatus and method for stimulating, recording and measuring facial expression. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27(1), 81–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lyttle, J.
(2001) The effectiveness of humor in persuasion: The case of business ethics training. The Journal of General Psychology, 128(2), 206–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macdonald, R. G., & Tatler, B. W.
(2018) Gaze in a real-world social interaction: A dual eye-tracking study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(10), 2162–2173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mackie, D. M., Silver, L., & Smith, E. R.
(2004) Emotion as an intergroup phenomenon. In Tiedens, L., & Leach, C. W. (Eds.), The social life of emotions (pp. 227-245). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macrae, C. N., Duffy, O. K., Miles, L. K., & Lawrence, J.
(2008) A case of hand waving: Action synchrony and person perception. Cognition, 109(1), 152–156. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mak, B. C. N., Liu, Y., & Deneen, C. C.
(2012) Humor in the workplace: A regulating and coping mechanism in socialization. Discourse & Communication, 6(2), 163–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mantovan, L., Giustolisi, B., & Panzeri, F.
(2019) Signing something while meaning its opposite: The expression of irony in Italian Sign Language (LIS). Journal of Pragmatics, 142, 47-61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marinkovic, K., Baldwin, S., Courtney, M. G., Witzel, T., Dale, A. M., & Halgren, E.
(2011) Right hemisphere has the last laugh: Neural dynamics of joke appreciationCognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11(1), 113–130.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Markson, L., & Paterson, K. B.
(2009) Effects of gaze-aversion on visual-spatial imaginationBritish Journal of Psychology, 100(3), 553–563. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, J. D., Wood, A., Cox, W. T. L., Sievert, S., Nowak, R., Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Zhao, F., Witkower, Z., Langbehn, A. T., & Niedenthal, P. M.
(2021) Evidence for distinct facial signals of reward, affiliation, and dominance from both perception and production tasksAffective Science, 2, 14–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. A.
(2007) The psychology of humor: An integrative approach. Elsevier Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. A., & Ford, T. E.
(2018) The psychology of humor: An integrative approach. 2nd edition. Elsevier Academic Press.Google Scholar
McIntosh, D. N.
(1996) Facial feedback hypotheses: Evidence, implications, and directions. Motivation and Emotion, 20(2), 121–147. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McSweeney, B.
(2009) Incoherent culture. European Journal of Cross-Cultural Competence and Management, 1(1), 22-27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mehoudar, E., Arizpe, J., Baker, C. I., & Yovel, G.
(2014) Faces in the eye of the beholder: Unique and stable eye scanning patterns of individual observers. Journal of vision, 14(7), 6-6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Messinger, D., & Fogel, A.
(2007) The interactive development of social smiling. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 327–366). Elsevier Academic Press. . DOI logo.Google Scholar
Messinger, D. S., Mattson, W. I., Mahoor, M. H., & Cohn, J. F.
(2012) The eyes have it: Making positive expressions more positive and negative expressions more negative. Emotion, 12(3), 430–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miellet, S., Vizioli, L., He, L., Zhou, X., & Caldara, R.
(2013) Mapping face recognition information use across cultures. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miles, L. K., Griffiths, J. L., Richardson, M. J., & Macrae, C. N.
(2010) Too late to coordinate: Contextual influences on behavioral synchrony. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(1), 52–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K., & Macrae, C. N.
(2009) The rhythm of rapport: Interpersonal synchrony and social perception. Cognition, 190, 585–589.
https://doi.org/
.Google Scholar
(2010) Moving Through TimePsychological science, 21(2), 222–223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, H. H., Graesser, A. C., & Louwerse, M. M.
(2010) The effect of context on humor: A constraint-based model of comprehending verbal jokes. Discourse Processes, 47(2), 104-129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitkidis, P., Mc Graw, J. J., Roepstorff, A., & Wallot, S.
(2015) Building trust: Heart rate synchrony and arousal during joint action increased by public goods game. Physiology & Behavior, 149, 101–106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morency, L. P., Christoudias, C. M., & Darrell, T.
(2006, November) Recognizing gaze aversion gestures in embodied conversational discourse. In Proceedings of the 8th international conference on Multimodal interfaces  (pp. 287–294). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mori, K., & Mori, H.
(2010) Examination of the passive facial feedback hypothesis using an implicit measure: With a furrowed brow, neutral objects with pleasant primes look less appealing. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 111(3), 785–789. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muecke, D. C.
(1978) Irony markers. Poetics, 7, 363–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nakassis, C., & Snedeker, J.
(2002) Beyond sarcasm: Intonation and context as relational cues in children’s recognition of irony. In Greenhyll, A., Hughs, M., Little Field, H., & Walsh, H. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-sixth Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 429–440). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Navarretta, C.
(2016) Mirroring facial expressions and emotions in dyadic conversations. In Proceedings of the Tenth International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2016), 469–474. European Language Resources Association. [URL].Google Scholar
Neal, D. T., & Chartrand, T. L.
(2011) Embodied emotion perception: Amplifying and dampening facial feedback modulates emotion perception accuracy. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(6), 673–678. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., Korb, S., Wood, A., & Rychlowska, M.
(2016). Revisiting the Simulation of Smiles model: The what, when, and why of mimicking smiles. In U. Hess & A. Fischer (Eds.), Studies in emotion and social interaction. Emotional mimicry in social context (p. 44–71). Cambridge University Press.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norrick, N.
(1993) Conversational joking: Humor in everyday talk. Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
(2001) On the conversational performance of narrative jokes: Toward an account of timing. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 14, 255–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norrick, N. R., & Spitz, A.
(2008) Humor as a resource for mitigating conflict in interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 40(10), 1661-1686. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Donnell-Trujillo, N., & Adams, K.
(1983) Heheh in conversation: some coordinating accomplishments of laughter. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 47, 175–191. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Olbrechts-Tyteca, L.
(1974) Le comique du discours. Editions de l’Université de Bruxelles.Google Scholar
Olkoniemi, H., Johander, E., & Kaakinen, J. K.
(2019) The role of look-backs in the processing of written sarcasm. Memory & Cognition, 47(1), 87–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Olkoniemi, H., Ranta, H., & Kaakinen, J. K.
(2016) Individual differences in the processing of written sarcasm and metaphor: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(3), 433.Google Scholar
Olkoniemi, H., & Kaakinen, J. K.
(2021) Processing of irony in text: A systematic review of eye-tracking studies. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/ Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oveis, C., Gruber, J., Keltner, D., Stamper, J. L., & Boyce, W. T.
(2009) Smile intensity and warm touch as thin slices of child and family affective style. Emotion, 9(4), 544–548. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Padilla, X.
(2012) ¿Existen rasgos prosódicos objetivos en los enunciados irónicos? Oralia, 14, 203–224.Google Scholar
Paggio, P., & Navarretta, C.
(2011) Head movements, facial expressions and feedback in danish first encounters interactions: A culture-specific analysis. In International Conference on Universal Access in Human-Computer Interaction (pp. 583-590). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papa, A., & Bonanno, G. A.
(2008) Smiling in the face of adversity: The interpersonal and intrapersonal functions of smiling. Emotion, 8, 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Partington, A.
(2007) Irony and reversal of evaluation. Journal of Pragmatics, 39, 1547–1569. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paxton, A., & Dale, R.
(2017) Interpersonal movement synchrony responds to high-and low-level conversational constraints. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 1135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013a) Argument disrupts interpersonal synchrony. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(11), 2092–2102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paxton, A., and Dale, R.
(2013b) Frame-differencing methods for measuring bodily synchrony in conversationBehavioral Research Methods, 45, 329–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paxton, A., Dale, R., & Richardson, D. C.
(2016). Social coordination of verbal and nonverbal behaviors. Routledge.Google Scholar
Pell, M. D., & Kotz, S. A.
(2011) On the time course of vocal emotion recognition. PLoS ONE, 6(11), e27256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peterson, M. F., Lin, J., Zaun, I., & Kanwisher, N.
(2016) Individual differences in face-looking behavior generalize from the lab to the world. Journal of Vision, 16(7), 12-12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P.
(2008) It’s fascinating research. The cognition of verbal irony. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(4), 286–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P. M., Zdrazilova, L., McConnachie, D., Deater-Deckard, K., & Petrill, S. A.
(2009) “That was smooth, Mom”: Children’s production of verbal and gestural irony. Metaphor and Symbol, 24, 237–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfeiffer, U., Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Jording, M., Bente, G., & Vogeley, K.
(2012) Eyes on the mind: Investigating the influence of gaze dynamics on the perception of others in real-time social interactionFrontiers in Psychology, 3, Article 537. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfeiffer, U. J., Vogeley, K., & Schilbach, L.
(2013) From gaze cueing to dual eye-tracking: novel approaches to investigate the neural correlates of gaze in social interaction. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10), 2516-2528. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, L., Corduas, M., Eisterhold, J., Seifried, B., Eggleston, A., & Attardo, S.
(2009) Prosodic markers of saliency in humorous narratives. Discourse Processes, 46, 517–540. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, L., Hu, G., & Baker, A.
(2012) The pragmatic function of intonation: Cueing agreement and disagreement in spoken English discourse and implications for ELT. In J. Romero-Trillo (Ed.), Pragmatics and prosody in English language teaching (pp. 199–218). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S.
(2004) Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(2), 169–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Prediction and embodiment in dialogueEuropean Journal of Social Psychology, 39(7), 1162–1168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) An integrated theory of language production and comprehensionThe Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 329–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piirainen-Marsh, A.
(2011) Irony and the moral order of secondary school classrooms. Linguistics and Education, 22(4), 364–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinar Sanz, M.
(Ed.) (2015) Multimodality and cognitive linguistics. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Platt, T., Hofmann, J., Ruch, W., & Proyer, R. T.
(2013) Duchenne display responses towards sixteen enjoyable emotions: Individual differences between no and fear of being laughed at. Motivation and Emotion, 37, 776–786. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plester, B. A., & Sayers, J.
(2007) “Taking the piss”: Functions of banter in the IT industryHUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 20(2), 157–187.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Priego-Valverde, B.
(2003). L’humour dans la conversation familière: Description et analyse linguistiques. Editions L’Harmattan.Google Scholar
(2006) How funny it is when everybody gets going! A case of co-construction of humor in conversationCírculo de Lingüística Aplicada a la Comunicación, 27, 72–100.Google Scholar
(2009) Failed humor in conversation: A double voicing analysis. In D. Chiaro & N. Norrick (Eds.), Humor in interaction (pp. 165–183). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Priego-Valverde, B., Bigi, B., Attardo, S., Pickering, L. & Gironzetti, E.
(2018) Is smiling during humor so obvious? A cross-cultural comparison of smiling behavior in humorous sequences in American English and French interactions. Intercultural Pragmatics, 15(4), 563-591. . DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Provine, R. R.
(2000) Laughter: A scientific investigation. Viking.Google Scholar
Quené, H., & Barthel, H.
(2015, August) Acoustic-phonetic properties of smiling revised – measurements on a natural video corpus. Paper presented at the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Glasgow.Google Scholar
Raidt, S., Bailly, G., & Elisei, F.
(2007) Gaze patterns during face-to-face interaction. In 2007 IEEE/WIC/ACM International Conferences on Web Intelligence and Intelligent Agent Technology-Workshops (pp. 338-341). Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers. [URL]. DOI logo
Raskin, V.
(1979) Semantic Mechanisms of Humor. Proceedings of the Fifth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 325-335. [URL]. DOI logo
(1985) Semantic mechanisms of humor. D. Reidel.Google Scholar
(2008) Theory of humor and practice of humor research: Editor’s notes and thoughts. In Raskin, V. (Ed.), The primer of humor research (pp. 1–16). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) A little metatheory: Thoughts on what a theory of computational humor should look like. In AAAI Technical Report FS-12-02, 62–67. [URL].Google Scholar
Raskin, V., Hempelmann, C. F., & Taylor, J. M.
(2009) How to understand and assess a theory: The evolution of the SSTH into the GTVH and now into the OSTH. Journal of Literary Theory, 3(2), 285–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K.
(Ed.) (1992) Eye movements and visual cognition. Scene perception and reading. Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raz, A.
(2004) Attention. In Spielberg, C. D. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of applied psychology (pp. 203–208). Elsevier Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reddish, P., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J.
(2013) Let’s dance together: Synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation. PLoS ONE, 8(8), e71182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richardson, D. C., & Dale, R.
(2005) Looking to understand: The coupling between speakers' and listeners' eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognitive Science, 29(6), 1045-1060. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richardson, D. C., Dale, R., & Kirkham, N. Z.
(2007) The art of conversation is coordination: common ground and the coupling of eye movements during dialogue. Psychological Science, 18(5), 407–413. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rigoulot, S., Wassiliwizky, E., & Pell, M. D.
(2013) Feeling backwards? How temporal order in speech affects the time course of vocal emotion recognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(367), 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robinson, J.
(2006) Managing trouble responsibility and relationships during conversational repair. Communication Monographs, 73(2), 137–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rockwell, P. A.
(2001) Facial expression and sarcasm. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93(1), 47–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) Sarcasm and other mixed messages: The ambiguous ways people use language. Edwin Mellen Press.Google Scholar
Rodríguez Mosquera, P. M. R., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S.
(2004) Inside the heart of emotion: On culture and relational concerns. In Tiedens, L., & Leach, C. W. (Eds.), The social life of emotions (pp. 187-202). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rogers, S. L., Speelman, C. P., Guidetti, O., & Longmuir, M.
(2018) Using dual eye tracking to uncover personal gaze patterns during social interaction. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1-9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosengrant, D., Hearrington, D., Alvarado, K., & Keeble, D.
(2011, August 3-4) Following Student Gaze Patterns in Physical Science Lectures. Paper presented at Physics Education Research Conference 2011, Omaha, Nebraska. Retrieved January 11, 2022, from [URL].
Rossano, F.
(2013) Gaze in conversation. In Sidnell, J. & Stivers, T. (Eds.), The handbook of conversation analysis (pp. 308-329). Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rubenstein, L.
(1969) Facial expressions: An objective method in the quantitative evaluation of emotional change. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 1, 305–306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruch, W.
(1993) Exhilaration and humor. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), The handbook of emotion (pp. 605–616). Guilford.Google Scholar
(1995) Will the real relationship between facial expression and affective experience please stand up: The case of exhilaration. Cognition and Emotion, 9(1), 33–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1998) The sense of humor: Explorations of a personality characteristic. Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
(2008) Psychology of humor. In V. Raskin (Ed.), The primer of humor research (pp. 17–100). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruch, W., & Rath, S.
(1993) The nature of humor appreciation: Toward an integration of perception of stimulus properties and affective experience. HUMOR – International Journal of Humor Research, 6(4), 363–384. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruiz-Belda, M. A., Fernández-Dols, J. M., Carrera, P., & Barchard, K.
(2003) Spontaneous facial expressions of happy bowlers and soccer fans. Cognition and Emotion, 17(2), 315-326. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruíz Gurillo, L.
(2012) La lingüística del humor en español. Arco/Libros.Google Scholar
(2021) Disrupted vs. sustained humor in colloquial conversations in peninsular Spanish. Journal of Pragmatics, 178, 162–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A.
(1995) Facial expressions of emotion: What lies beyond minimal universality?. Psychological bulletin, 118(3), 379. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruvolo, P., Messinger, D., & Movellan, J.
(2015) Infants time their smiles to make their moms smile. PLoS one, 10(9), e0136492. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rychlowska, M., Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., Schyns, P. G., Martin, J. D. & Niedenthal, P.
(2017) Functional smiles: Tools for love, sympathy, and war. Psychological Science, 28(9), 1259–1270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sacks, H.
(1974/1989) An analysis of the course of a joke’s telling in conversation. In R. Bauman. & J. Sherzer (Eds.), Explorations in the ethnography of speaking (pp. 337–353). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Salvucci, D. D., & Goldberg, J. H.
(2000) Identifying fixations and saccades in eye tracking protocols. In A. T. Duchowski (Ed.), ETRA ’00 proceedings of the 2000 symposium on eye tracking research & applications (pp. 71–78). Association for Computing Machinery. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Samson, A. C., & Gross, J. J.
(2012) Humour as emotion regulation: The differential consequences of negative versus positive humourCognition and Emotion, 26, 375–384. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Kaiser, S., Wehrle, T. & Scherer, K. R.
(2007) Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotionEuropean Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 470–480. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scarantino, A.
(2017) How to do things with emotional expressions. The theory of affective pragmatics. Psychological Inquiry, 28(2-3), 165–185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schaffer, R. R.
(1982) Vocal cues for irony in English (Doctoral dissertation, The Ohio State University).Google Scholar
Scharrer, L., Christmann, U., & Knoll, M.
(2011) Voice modulations in German ironic speech. Language and Speech, 54(4), 435–465. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schiffrin, D.
(1987) Discourse markers. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schlesinger, I., & Hurvitz, S.
(2008) The structure of misunderstandings. Pragmatics and Cognition, 16(3), 568–585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, K. L., Ambadar, Z., Cohn, J. F., & Reed, L. I.
(2006) Movement differences between deliberate and spontaneous facial expressions: Zygomaticus major action in smiling. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30, 37–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, K. L., Bhattacharya, S., & Denlinger, R.
(2009) Comparison of deliberate and spontaneous facial movement in smiles and eyebrow raises. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33, 35–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schützwohl, A., & Reisenzein, R.
(2012) Facial expressions in response to a highly surprising event exceeding the field of vision: a test of Darwin's theory of surprise. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(6), 657-664. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N.
(1990) Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In E. T. Higgins & R. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 527-561). Guilford.Google Scholar
Seder, P. J., & Oishi, S.
(2012) Intensity of smiling in Facebook photos predicts future life satisfaction. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(4), 407–413. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seltman, H. J.
(2015) Experimental design and analysis. Carnegie Mellon University.Google Scholar
Seyfeddinipur, M., & Kita, S.
(2001) Gestures and self-monitoring in speech production. Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 27(1), 457–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shapiro, L.
2011Embodied cognition. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Shively, R.
(2018) Learning and using conversational humor in a second language during study abroad. De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shockley, K., & Riley, M. A.
(2015) Interpersonal couplings in human interactions. In C. L. W. Jr & N. Marwan (Eds.), Recurrence quantification analysis (pp. 399–421). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shockley, K., Santana, M. V., & Fowler, C. A.
(2003) Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversationJournal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(2), 326–332. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shor, R. E.
(1978) The production and judgment of smile magnitude. Journal of General Psychology, 98, 79–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simarro Vázquez, M., El Khatib, N., Hamrick, P., & Attardo, S.
Slepian, M. L., Weisbuch, M., O Rule, N., & Ambady N.
(2011) Tough and tender: Embodied categorization of gender. Psychological Science, 22(1), 26–8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smilek, D., Birmingham, E., Cameron, D., Bischof, W., & Kingstone, A.
(2006) Cognitive ethology and exploring attention in real-world scenes. Brain Research, 1080, 101–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soussignan, R., Nadel, J., Canet, P., & Girardin, P.
(2002) Sensitivity to social contingency and positive emotion in 2-month-olds. Infancy, 10(2), 123–144 (2006) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S.
(1988) Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 208–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tabacaru, S.
(2020) Faces of sarcasm. Exploring raised eyebrows with sarcasm in French political debates. In Athanasiadou, A., & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), The diversity of irony (pp. 256–277). Mouton De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tabacaru, S., & Lemmens, M.
(2014) Raised eyebrows as gestural triggers in humour: The case of sarcasm and hyper-understanding. The European Journal of Humour Research, 2(2), 11–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tannen, D.
(1984) Conversational style analyzing talk among friends. Ablex Publishing Corporation.Google Scholar
Taras, V., Steel, P., & Kirkman, B. L.
(2016) Does country equate with culture? Beyond geography in the search for cultural boundaries. Management International Review, 56(4), 455-487. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tartter, V. C.
(1980) Happy talk: Perceptual and acoustic affects of smiling on speech. Perception and Psychophysics, 27, 24–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, D., Mackenzie, I. G., Leuthold, H., & Filik, R.
(2016) Emotional responses to irony and emoticons in written language: Evidence from EDA and facial EMGPsychophysiology, 53(7), 1054–1062.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, J.
(1941) Development of facial expression of emotion in blind and seeing children [Doctoral Dissertation, Columbia University]. Archives of Psychology, 264. [URL].Google Scholar
Torre, I.
(2014) Production and perception of smiling voice. In Lee, T. (Ed.), Proceedings of the first Postgraduate and Academic Researchers in Linguistics conference at York (PARLAY 2013) (pp. 100–117). University of York.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W.
(2004) Show your pride: Evidence for a discrete emotion expression. Psychological science, 15(3), 194-197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trofimovich, P.
(2013) Interactive alignment: Implications for the teaching and learning of second language pronunciation. In J. Levis & K. LeVelle (Eds.). Proceedings of the 4th Pronunciation in Second Language Learning and Teaching Conference. (pp. 1–9). Iowa State University.Google Scholar
Tsakona, V.
(2007) Towards a revised typology of humorous texts and humorous lines. In Popa, D. & Attardo, S. (Eds.), New approaches to the linguistics of humor (pp. 35–43). Editura Academica Galaţi.Google Scholar
(2020) Recontextualizing humor. Rethinking the analysis and teaching of humor. De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tschacher, W., Rees, G. M., & Ramseyer, F.
(2014) Nonverbal synchrony and affect in dyadic interactions. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 1323. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Țurcan, A., & Filik, R.
(2016) An eye-tracking investigation of written sarcasm comprehension: The roles of familiarity and context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(12), 1867–1893.
https://doi.org/
.Google Scholar
(2017) Investigating sarcasm comprehension using eye-tracking during reading: What are the roles of literality, familiarity, and echoic mention. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. L. (Eds), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 255-276). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Urios-Aparisi, E., & Wagner, M. M.
(2011) Prosody of humor in Sex and the CityPragmatics & Cognition, 19(3), 507–529. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Utsumi, A.
(2000) Verbal irony as implicit display of ironic environment: Distinguishing ironic utterances from nonironyJournal of Pragmatics, 32(12), 1777–1806. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., & De Steno, D.
(2010) The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 693–695. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valtakari, N. V., Hooge, I. T. C., Viktorsson, C., Nyström, P., Falck-Ytter, T., & Hessels, R.
(2021). Eye tracking in human interaction: Possibilities and limitationsBehavior Research Methods. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Beek, Y., Van Dolderen, M. S., & Demon Dubas, J. J.
(2006) Gender-specific development of nonverbal behaviours and mild depression in adolescenceJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(12), 1272-1283. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Varlet, M., Marin, L., Lagarde, J., & Bardy, B. G.
(2011) Social postural coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 37(2), 473–483. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vertegaal, R., Slagter, R., Van der Veer, G., & Nijholt, A.
(2001) Eye gaze patterns in conversations: There is more to conversational agents than meets the eyes. In Jacko, J., & Sears, A. (Eds.), Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 301-308). Association for Computing Machinery. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Võ, M. L. H., Smith, T. J., Mital, P. K., & Henderson, J. M.
(2012) Do the eyes really have it? Dynamic allocation of attention when viewing moving faces. Journal of Vision, 12(13), 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vranjes, J., Bot, H., Feyaerts, K., & Brône, G.
Vranjes, J., Brône, G., & Feyaerts, K.
(2018) Dual feedback in interpreter-mediated interactions: on the role of gaze in the production of listener responsesJournal of Pragmatics, 134, 15–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vrij, A.
(2002) Telling and detecting lies. Applying psychology, 179-241.Google Scholar
Vrij, A., Edward, K., & Bull, R.
(2001) Stereotypical verbal and nonverbal responses while deceiving othersPersonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(7), 899–909. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wagner, H. L., & Smith, J.
(1991) Facial expression in the presence of friends and strangers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15(4), 201-214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, S. F., Liu, Z. L., Wang, Z. Y., Wu, G. B., Shen, P. J., He, S., & Wang, X.
(20130) Analyses of a multimodal spontaneous facial expression database. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 4, 34–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Washburn, R. W.
(1929) A study of the smiling and laughing of infants in the first year of lifeGenetic Psychology Monographs, 6, 403–537.Google Scholar
Weigand, E.
(1999) Misunderstanding: The standard case. Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 763–785. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wennerstrom, A.
(2001) The music of everyday speech: Prosody and discourse analysis. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Whalen, J. M., & Pexman, P. M.
(2010) How do children respond to verbal irony in face-to-face communication? The development of mode adoption across middle childhoodDiscourse Processes, 47(5), 363–387.  DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Humor support and mode adoption. In Attardo, S. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 371–384). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wickberg, D.
(1998) The senses of humor, self and laughter in modern America. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Wild, B., Erb, M., Eyb, M., Bartels, M., & Grodd, W.
(2003) Why are smiles contagious? An fMRI study of the interaction between perception of facial affect and facial movements. Psychiatry Research, 123(1), 17–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A.
(2008) Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmthScience, 322(5901), 606–607. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. A., Burns, E. L., & Harmon, E. A.
(2009) Insincere utterances and gaze: eye contact during sarcastic statements. Perceptual and motor skills, 108(2), 565-572. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, A. D., & Golonka, S.
(2013) Embodied cognition is not what you think it isFrontiers in psychology, 4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M.
(2002) Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 625–636. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, R. A., & Foglia, L.
(2017) Embodied cognition. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. [URL].Google Scholar
Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C.
(2009) Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20, 1–5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wolff, H. A., Smith, C. E., & Murray, H. A.
(1934) The psychology of humor. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 28(4), 341–365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Woodzicka, J. A., & LaFrance, M.
(2001) Real versus imagined gender harassment. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 15–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yarbus, A. L.
(1967) Eye movements and vision. Plenum Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Young, G., & Décarie, T. G.
(1977) An ethology-based catalogue of facial/vocal behaviour in infancy. Animal Behaviour, 25, 95-107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yus, F.
(2003) Humor and the search for relevanceJournal of Pragmatics, 35(9), 1295–1331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). Humour and relevance. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Relevance-theoretic treatments of humor. In Attardo, S. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 189–203). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. R., & Komer, M. J.
(1971) Infant smiling to nonsocial stimuli and the recognition hypothesis. Child Development, 42(5), 1327–1339. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhang, X., Sugano, Y., Fritz, M., & Bulling, A.
(2015) Appearance-based gaze estimation in the wild. In Proceedings of the IEEE conference on computer vision and pattern recognition (pp. 4511-4520). Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar