Part of
Figurative Thought and Language in Action
Edited by Mario Brdar and Rita Brdar-Szabó
[Figurative Thought and Language 16] 2022
► pp. 167184
References
Anderson, M. L.
(2010) Neural re-use as a fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 245–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bak, T. H., & Hodges, J. R.
(2003) The effects of motor neuron disease on language: Further evidence. Brain and Language, 89, 354–361. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergen, B. K.
(2012) Louder than words: The new science of how the mind makes meaning. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L.
(2015) Using Figurative language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017, April) Embodied simulations and verbal irony. The 3rd International Symposium on Figurative Thought and Language, Osijek, Croatia.Google Scholar
(2018, June) Embodied meaning in embedded metaphor: Variable pragmatic effects of metaphorical and non-metaphorical proverbs. Researching and Applying Metaphor 12 (RaAM-12), Hong Kong, China.Google Scholar
(in press). How language makes meaning: Embodiment and conjoined antonymy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logo
Craddock, T. J. A., Tuszynski, J. A., & Hameroff, S.
(2012) Cytoskeletal signaling: Is memory encoded in microtubule lattices by CaMKII phosphorylation? PLoS Computational Biology, 8(3), e1002421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Vega, M.
(2008) Levels of embodied meaning: From pointing to counterfactuals. In M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg and A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols, Embodiment and Meaning, (pp. 285–308). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V.
(2007) Before and below theory of mind: Embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 659–669. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Mirror neurons and the social nature of language: The neural exploitation hypothesis. Social Neuroscience, 3, 317–333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V., & Lakoff, G.
(2005) The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in reason and language. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(3), 455–479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., & Colston, H. L.
(Eds.) (2007) Irony in language and thought: A cognitive science reader. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Taylor & Francis Group. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glass, A. L.
(2016) Cognition: A neuroscience approach. Cambridge, U.K: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P.
(2002) Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(3), 558–565. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M., Sato, M., & Cattaneo, L.
(2008) Use-induced motor plasticity affects the processing of abstract and concrete language. Current Biology, 18(7), R290–R291. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Kelly, S. D., & Wagner, S.
(2001) Explaining math: Gesturing lightens the load. Psychological Science, 12(6), 516-522. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2001) The resilience of gesture in talk: Gesture in blind speakers and listeners. Developmental Science, 4(4), 416–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M.
(1998) Why do we gesture when we speak? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7(2), 54–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeil, D.
(1992) Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L.
(2007) Language and mirror neurons. In G. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sedivy, J.
(2014) Language and mind: An introduction to psycholinguistics. Oxford, U.K: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Warren, P.
(2013) Introducing psycholinguistics. Cambridge, U.K: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., & Taylor, L. J.
(2006) Seeing, acting, understanding: Motor resonance in language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(1), 1–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar