Gestural symbolic strategies in children with Down syndrome
In early communicative development, children with Down syndrome (DS) make extensive use of gestures to compensate for articulatory difficulties. Here, we analyzed the symbolic strategies that underlie this gesture production, compared to that used by typically developing children. Using the same picture-naming task, 79 representational gestures produced by 10 children with DS and 42 representational gestures produced by 10 typically developing children of comparable developmental age (3;1 vs. 2;9, respectively) were collected. The gestures were analyzed and classified according to four symbolic strategies. The two groups performed all of the strategies, with no significant differences for either choice or frequency of the strategies used. The item analysis highlighted that some photographs tended to elicit the use of the same strategy in both groups. These results indicate that similar symbolic strategies are active in children with DS as in typically developing children, which suggests interesting similarities in their symbolic development.
Article outline
- Introduction
- Symbolic and gestural abilities in typically developing children
- Symbolic and gestural abilities in children with Down syndrome
- Aims of the present study
- Methods
- Participants
- Material and procedures
- The picture-naming game
- The Italian MB-CDI
- Coding
- Gesture identification
- Symbolic strategies
- Reliability
- Statistical analysis
- Results
- Lexical accuracy and gestural production through the MB-CDI and PiNG
- Distribution of symbolic strategies
- The item effect in the use of the different symbolic strategies
- Discussion
- Lexical accuracy and gestural production
- Symbolic strategies: Distributions and item effects
- Limitations of the present study
- Conclusion
-
References
References (71)
References
Abbeduto, Leonard, Warren, Steven F., & Conners, Frances A. (2007). Language development in Down syndrome: from the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 13 (3), 247–261.
Arbib, Michael A., Ganesh, Varsha, & Gasser, Brad. (2014). Dyadic brain modeling, ontogenetic ritualization of gesture in apes, and the contributions of primate mirror neuron systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369 (1644), 20130414.
Bates, Elizabeth, Benigni, Laura, Bretherton, Inge, Camaioni, Luigia, & Volterra, Virginia. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. NewYork: Academic Press.
Bates, Elizabeth, Camaioni, Luigia, & Volterra, Virginia. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merril Palmer Quarterly, 211, 205–226. Retrieved November 2, 2020, from [URL]
Beeghly, Marjorie, Weiss-Perry, Bedonna, & Cicchetti, Dante. (1990). Beyond sensorimotor functioning: Early communicative and play development of children with Down syndrome. In Dante Cicchetti & Marjorie Beeghly. (Eds.), Children with Down syndrome. A developmental perspective (pp. 329–368). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bello, Arianna, Caselli, Maria Cristina, Pettenati, Paola, & Stefanini, Silvia. (2010). Parole in gioco – PinG. Una prova di comprensione e produzione lessicale. Firenze OS.
Bello, Arianna, Giannantoni, Patrizia, Pettenati, Paola, Stefanini, Silvia, & Caselli, Maria Cristina. (2012). Assessing lexicon: validation and developmental data of the Picture Naming Game (PiNG), a new picture naming task for toddlers. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 471, 589–602.
Bello, Arianna, Onofrio, Daniela, & Caselli, Maria Cristina. (2014). Nouns and predicates comprehension, and production in children with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 351, 761–775.
Berglund, Eva, Eriksson, Mårten, & Johansson, Iréne. (2001). Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 441, 179–191.
Boyatzis, Chris. J. & Watson, Malcolm W. (1993). Preschool children’s symbolic representation of objects through gestures. Child Development, 641, 729–735.
Brentari, Diane, Di Renzo, Alessio, Keane, Jonathan, & Volterra, Virginia. (2015). Cognitive, cultural, and linguistic sources of a handshape distinction expressing agentivity. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7 (1), 95–123.
Capirci, Olga, Iverson, Jana M., Pizzuto, Elena, & Volterra, Virginia. (1996). Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child language, 23 (3), 645–673.
Capone, Nina C. (2007). Tapping toddlers’ evolving semantic representation via gesture. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 501, 732–745.
Capone, Nina. C. & McGregor, Karla K. (2004). Gesture development: a review for clinical and research practices. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47 (1), 173–186.
Cárdenas, Karina. (2012). Primeros usos simbólicos de niños con síndrome de Down en contextos de interacción triádica: Un estudio longitudinal entre los 12 y 21 meses de edad. Doctoral dissertation, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Facultad de Psicología.
Cárdenas, Karina, Rodríguez, Cintia, & Palacios, Pedro. (2014). First symbols in a girl with Down syndrome: A longitudinal study from 12 to 18 months. Infant Behavior & Development, 371, 416–427.
Caselli, Maria Cristina, Bello, Arianna, Pasqualetti, Patrizio, Rinaldi, Pasquale, & Stefanini, Silvia. (2015). Il primo vocabolario del bambino: Gesti, parole e frasi. Forme lunghe e forme brevi del questionario e valori di riferimento per la fascia 8–36 mesi. Milano: Franco Angeli.
Caselli, Maria Cristina, Rinaldi, Pasquale, Stefanini, Silvia, & Volterra, Virginia. (2012). Early action and gesture ‘vocabulary’ and its relation with word comprehension and production. Child Development, 831, 526–542.
Caselli, Maria Cristina, Vicari, Stefano, Longobardi, Emiddia, Lami, Laura, Pizzoli, Carla, & Stella, Giacomo. (1998). Gestures and words in early development of children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 411, 1125–1135.
Cattani, Allegra, Floccia, Caroline, Kidd, Evan, Pettenati, Paola, Onofrio, Daniela, & Volterra, Virginia. (2019). Gestures and words in naming: evidence from crosslinguistic and crosscultural comparisons. Language Learning, 69 (3), 709–746.
Chapman, Robin S., Hesketh, Linda J., & Kistler, Doris J. (2002). Predicting longitudinal change in language production and comprehension in individuals with Down syndrome: hierarchical modeling. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 451, 902–915.
Clibbens, John. (2001). Signing and lexical development in children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 7 (3), 101–105.
Couzens, Donna, Cuskelly, Monica, & Haynes, Michele. (2011). Cognitive development and Down syndrome: age-related change on the Stanford-Binet Test. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 116 (3), 181–204.
Deckers, Stijn R., Van Zaalen, Yvonne, Mens, E. J., Van Balkom, Hans, & Verhoeven, Ludo. (2016). The concurrent and predictive validity of the Dutch version of the Communicative Development Inventory in children with Down Syndrome for the assessment of expressive vocabulary in verbal and signed modalities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 561, 99–107.
Galeote, Miguel, Sebastián, Eugenia, Checa, Elena, Rey, Rocío, & Soto, Pilar. (2011). The development of vocabulary in Spanish children with Down syndrome: comprehension, production, and gestures. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 36 (3), 184–196.
Galeote, Miguel, Soto, Pilar, Checa, Eugenia, Gómez, Aaron, & Lamela, Elena. (2008). The acquisition of productive vocabulary in Spanish children with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 33 (4) 292–302.
Grieco, Julie, Pulsifer, Margaret, Seligsohn, Karen, Skotko, Brian, & Schwartz, Alison. (2015). Down syndrome: cognitive and behavioural functioning across the lifespan. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C: Seminars in Medical Genetics, 169 (2), 135–149.
Griffith, Rachel. (2006).
Griffiths Mental Development Scales – Extended Revised GMDS-ER. Firenze: Giunti O.S.
Hostetter, Autumn B. & Alibali, Martha W. (2008). Visible embodiment: gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 151, 495–514.
Iverson, Jana M., Capirci, Olga, & Caselli, Maria Cristina. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9 (1), 23–43.
Iverson, Jana M., Longobardi, Emiddia, & Caselli, Maria Cristina. (2003). Relationship between gestures and words in children with Down’s syndrome and typically developing children in the early stages of communicative development. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 381, 179–197.
Kendon, Adam. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kim, Hyo I., Seong, Woo K., Jiyong, Kim, Ra, Jeon Ha, & Wa, Jung Da. (2017). Motor and cognitive developmental profiles in children with Down syndrome. Annals of Rehabilitation Medicine, 41 (1), 97–103.
Kita, Sotaro, & Özyürek, Aslı. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 481, 16–32.
Launonen, Kaisa. (1996). Enhancing communication skills of children with Down syndrome: early use of manual signs. In Stephen Tetzchner & Mogens Hygum Jensen. (Eds.), Augmentative and alternative communication: European perspectives (Chapter 13, 213–231). London: Whurr.
Lavelli, Manuela, & Majorano, Marinella. (2016). Spontaneous gesture production and lexical abilities in children with specific language impairment in a naming task. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 591, 784–796.
Marentette, Paula, Pettenati, Paola, Bello, Arianna, & Volterra, Virginia. (2016). Gesture and symbolic representation in Italian and English-speaking Canadian two-year-old. Child Development, 87 (3), 944–961.
McNeill, David. (1992). Hand and mind. What the hands reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Miller, Jon. F., Sedey, Allison L., & Miolo, Giuliana. (1995). Validity of parent report measures of vocabulary development for children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 38 (5), 1037–1044.
O’Toole, Ciara & Chiat, Shula. (2006). Symbolic functioning and language development in children with Down syndrome. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41 (2), 155–171.
Overton, Willis F. & Jackson, Joseph P. (1973). The representation of hand-as-hands in action sequences: a developmental study. Child Development, 441, 309–314.
Özçalişkan, Şeyda. (2016). Do gestures follow speech in bilinguals’ description of motion? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (3), 644–653.
Özçaliskan, Şeyda & Dimitrova, Nevena. (2013). How gesture input provides a helping hand to language development. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34 (4), 227–236.
Özçalışkan, Şeyda & Goldin-Meadow, Susan. (2009). When gesture-speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24 (2), 190–217.
Pettenati, Paola, Sekine, Kazuki, Congestrì, Elena, & Volterra, Virginia. (2012). A comparative study on representational gestures in Italian and Japanese children. Journal of Non-Verbal Behaviour, 361, 149–164.
Pettenati, Paola, Stefanini, Silvia, & Volterra, Virginia. (2010). Motoric characteristics of representational gestures produced by young children in a naming task. Journal of Child Language, 37 (4), 887–911.
Rinaldi, Pasquale. (2008). Competenze lessicali di bambini sordi bilingui in età prescolare / Lexical competencies in bilingual deaf preschoolers. Journal of Applied Psycholinguistics, 8 (3), 93–107.
Rinaldi, Pasquale, Caselli, Maria Cristina, Di Renzo, Alessio, Gulli, Tiziana, & Volterra, Virginia. (2014). Sign vocabulary in deaf toddlers exposed to sign language since birth. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 19 (3), 303–318.
Roid, Gale H. & Miller, Lucy J. (1997). Leiter International Performance Scale – Revised, Firenze: Giunti O.S.
Ruskin, Ellen, Mundy, Peter, Kasari, Connie, & Sigman, Marian. (1994). Object mastery motivation in children with DS. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 981, 499–509.
Sachet, Alison B. & Mottweiler, Candice M. (2013). The distinction between role-play and object substitution in pretend play. In Marjorie Taylor. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of the development of the imagination (pp. 175–185). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sansavini, Alessandra, Bello, Arianna, Guarini, Annalisa, Savini, Silvia, Alessandroni, Rosina, Faldella, Giacomo, & Caselli, Maria Cristina. (2015). Noun and predicate comprehension/production and gestures in extremely preterm children at two years of age: Are they delayed? Journal of Communication Disorders, 581, 126–142.
Sigman, Marian & Sena, Rhonda. (1993). Pretend play in high-risk and developmentally delayed children. In Marc. Bornstein & Anne Watson O’Reilly. (Eds.), New directions for child development: The role of play in the development of thought (Vol. 591, pp. 29–42). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Singh, Joginder, Teresa, Iacono, & Gray, Kylie M. (2014). An investigation of the intentional communication and symbolic play skills of children with Down syndrome and cerebral palsy in Malaysia. Journal of Early Intervention, 36 (2), 71–89.
Stefanini, Silvia, Bello, Arianna, Caselli, Maria Cristina, Iverson, Jana M., & Volterra, Virginia. (2009). Co-speech gestures in a naming task: developmental data. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24 (2), 168–189.
Stefanini, Silvia, Caselli, Maria Cristina, & Volterra, Virginia. (2007). Spoken and gestural production in a naming task by young children with Down syndrome. Brain and Language, 1011, 208–221.
Tomasello, Michael. (2008). The origin of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Venuti, Paola, De Falco, Simona, Esposito, Gianluca, & Bornstein, Marc H. (2009). Mother-child play in children with Down syndrome and typical development. American Journal on Intellectual and Development Disabilities, 114 (4), 274–288.
Vietze, Peter M. (1983). Attention and exploratory behaviour in infant with Down syndrome. In Tiffany Field & Anita Sostek. (Eds.), Infants born at risk: Psychological perceptual and cognitive processes (pp. 251–268). Philadelphia: Grune & Stratton.
Volterra, Virginia, Capirci, Olga, Caselli, Maria Cristina, Rinaldi, Pasquale, & Sparaci, Laura. (2017). Developmental evidence for continuity from action to gesture to sign/word. Language, Interaction and Acquisition, 8 (1), 13–41.
Werner, Heinz & Kaplan, Bernard. (1963). Symbol formation: An organismic-developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Wright, Courtney A., Kaiser, Ann P., Reikowsky, Dawn I., & Roberts, Megan Y. (2013). Effects of a naturalistic sign intervention on expressive language of toddlers with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56 (3), 994–1008.
Zampini, Laura & D’Odorico, Laura. (2013). Vocabulary development in children with Down syndrome: Longitudinal and cross-sectional data. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 38 (4), 310–317.
Zukow-Goldring, Patricia. (2012). Assisted imitation: first steps in the seed model of language development. Language Sciences, 341, 569–582.
Zukow-Goldring, Patricia & Arbib, Michael A. (2007). Affordances, effectivities, and assisted imitation: caregivers and the directing of attention. Neurocomputing, 70 (13–15), 2181–2193.
Cited by (2)
Cited by two other publications
Capirci, Olga, Morgana Proietti & Virginia Volterra
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 4 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.