Article published In:
Gesture
Vol. 22:1 (2023) ► pp.3961
References
Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., & Smith, C. B.
(1998) Gestures, words, and early object sharing. In V. Volterra, & Erting, J. C. (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 31–41). Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, D., & Reilly, J.
(2002) MacArthur Child Development Inventory: normative data for American Sign Language. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 7 (2), 83–106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baker, A., Van den Bogaerde, B., & Woll, B.
(2008) Methods and procedures in sign language acquisition studies. In A. Baker & B. Woll (Eds.), Sign Language Acquisition (pp. 1–50). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V.
(1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 21 (3), 205–226.Google Scholar
Brooks, R., Singleton, J., & Meltzoff, A.
(2020) Enhanced gaze-following behavior in deaf infants of deaf parents. Developmental Science, 23 (e12900). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G.
(1998) Origins of joint visual attention in infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63 1, 144–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Iverson, J., Pizzuto, E., & Volterra, V.
(1996) Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language, 23 1, 645–673. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Montanari, S., & Volterra, V.
(1998) Gestures, signs, and words in early language development. New directions for child and adolescent development, 79 1, 45–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Iverson, J., Montanari, S., & Volterra, V.
(2002) Gestural, signed and spoken modalities in early language development: the role of linguistic input. Bilingualism: language and cognition, 5 (1), 25–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Contaldo, A., Caselli, M. C., & Volterra, V.
Capirci, O., & Volterra, V.
Caselli, N., Lieberman, A., & Pyers, J.
(2020) The ASL-CDI 2.0 An updated, normed adaptation of the MacArthur Bates Communicative Development Inventory for American Sign Language. Behavior Research Methods, 52 1, 2071–2084. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J., Koster, I., & Noom, M. J.
(2010) The relationship between pointing and language development: a meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 30 1, 352–366. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Raeve, L.
(2016) Cochlear implants in Belgium: Prevalence in pediatric and adult cochlear implantation. European Annals of Otorhinolaryngology, Head Neck Diseases, 133 (1), S57–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A.
(1978) Language acquisition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Demey, E., Van Herreweghe, M., & Vermeerbergen, M.
(2008) Iconicity in sign languages. In K. Willems & L. De Cuypere (Eds.), Naturalness and iconicity in language (pp. 189–214). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Easterbrooks, S., & Baker, S.
(2002) Language learning in children who are deaf and hard of hearing: multiple pathways. Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S.
(1999) The role of gesture in communication and thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3 (11), 419–429. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Pointing sets the stage for learning language and creating language. Child Development, 78 (3), 741–745. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, M., & Chasin, J.
(2005) Visual attention in deaf and hearing infants: the role of auditory cues. Journal of Child psychology and Psychiatry, 46 (10). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoiting, N.
(2006) Deaf children are verb attenders: early sign language acquisition in Dutch toddlers. In B. Schick, M. Marschark, & P. E. Spencer (Eds.), Advances in sign language development by deaf children (pp. 161–188). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hoiting, N., & Slobin, D. I.
(2002) What a deaf child needs to see: advantages of a natural sign language over a sign system. In R. Schulmeister, & H. Reinitzer (Eds.), Progress in sign language research. In honor of Siegmund Prillwitz / Fortschritte in der Gebärdensprachforschung. Festschrift für Siegmund Prillwitz (pp. 268–277). Signum.Google Scholar
Iverson, J.
(2010) Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language, 37 (2), 229–261. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J., Capirci, O., & Caselli, M. C.
(1994) From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9(1), 23–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2005) Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16 (5), 367–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J., Capirci, O., Volterra, V., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2008) Learning to talk in a gesture-rich world: early communication in Italian vs. American children. First Language, 28 (2), 164–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kanto, L., Laakso, M.-L., & Huttunen, K.
(2015) Differentiation in language and gesture use during early bilingual development of hearing children of deaf parents. Bilingualism: language and cognition, 18 (4), 769–788. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, A., Hatrak, M., & Mayberry, R. I.
(2014) Learning to look for language: development of joint attention in young deaf children. Language Learning and Development, 10 (1), 19–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lillo-Martin, D., de Quadros, R. M., Chen Pichler, D., & Fieldsteel, Z.
(2014) Language choice in bimodal bilingual development. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 (1163), 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lüke, C., Leinweber, J., & Ritterfeld, U.
(2019) Walking, pointing, talking – the predictive value of early walking and pointing behavior for later language skills. Journal of Child Language, 46 (6), 1228–1237. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meristo, M., & Strid, K.
(2020) Language first: deaf children from deaf families spontaneously anticipate false beliefs. Journal of Cognition and Development, 21 (4), 622–630. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R., & Karchmer, M.
(2004) Chasing the mythical ten percent: parental hearing status of deaf and hard of hearing students in the United States. Sign Language Studies, 4 1, 138–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgenstern, A., Caët, S., Collombel-Leroy, M., Limousin, F., & Blondel, M.
Mouvet, K.
(2013) What with sign language? A longitudinal study of the language development of young deaf children in Flanders in times of cochlear implantation. [Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ghent University].
Mouvet, K., Matthijs, L., Loots, G., Taverniers, M., & Van Herreweghe, M.
(2013) The language development of a deaf child with a cochlear implant. Language Sciences, 35 1, 59–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalişkan, S. & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2010) Sex differences in language first appear in gesture. Developmental Science, 13 (5), 752–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P.
(2012) Use of Sign Space. In R. Pfau, M. Steinbach, & B. Woll (Eds.), Sign Language: an international handbook (pp. 412–431). De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P., & Vigliocco, G.
(2014) The bridge of iconicity: from a world of experience to the experience of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369 1, 20130300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pichler, C.
(2012) Acquisition. In R. Pfau, M. Steinbach, & B. Woll (Eds.), Sign Language: An international handbook (pp. 647–686). De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salo, V. C., Reeb-Sutherland, B., Frenkel, T. I., Bowman, L. C., & Rowe, M. L.
(2019) Does intention matter? Relations between parent pointing, infant pointing, and developing language ability. Journal of the Cognitive Development Society, 20 (5), 635–655. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slonimska, A., Özyürek, A., & Capirci, O.
(2020) The role of iconicity and simultaneity for efficient communication: the case of Italian Sign Language (LIS). Cognition, 200 1, 104246. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U.
(2007) A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78 (3), 705–722. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van den Bogaerde, B.
(2000) Input and interaction in deaf families. LOT Publications. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., & Sparaci, L.
(2017) Developmental evidence for continuity from action to gesture to sign/word. Language, interaction and acquisition, 8 (1), 13–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Rinaldi, P., & Sparaci, L.
(2018) From action to spoken and signed language through gesture. Interaction Studies, 19 (2), 216–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wille, B., Mouvet, K., Van Lierde, K., & Van Herreweghe, M.
Wille, B., Van Lierde, K., & Van Herreweghe, M.
(2019) Parental strategies used in communication with their deaf infants. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 35 (2), 165–183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wittenburg, P., Brugman, H., Russel, A., Klassmann, A., & Sloetjes, H.
(2006) ELAN: a professional framework for multimodality research. Proceedings of LREC 2006, Fifth Language Resources and Evaluation Conference (pp. 1556–1559).Google Scholar
Woolfe, T., Herman, R., Roy, P., & Woll, B.
(2010) Early vocabulary development in deaf native signers: a British Sign Language adaptation of the child development inventory. Journal of Child psychology and Psychiatry, 51 (3), 322–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zukow-Goldring, P.
(2012) Assisted imitation: first steps in the seed model of language development. Language Sciences, 34 (5), 569–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar