Article published In:
Gesture
Vol. 22:1 (2023) ► pp.94114
References (70)
References
Adorni, R., Manfredi, M., & Proverbio, A. M. (2013). Since when or how often? dissociating the roles of age of acquisition (AoA) and lexical frequency in early visual word processing. Brain and Language, 124 (1), 132–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Kita, S., & Young, A. J. (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15 (6), 593–613. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arslan, B., Aktan-Erciyes, A., & Göksun, T. (2023). Multimodal language in bilingual and monolingual children: Gesture production and speech disfluency. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1–13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Azar, Z., Backus, A., & Özyürek, A. (2020). Language contact does not drive gesture transfer: Heritage speakers maintain language specific gesture patterns in each language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23 (2), 414–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beattie, G. & Coughlan, J. (1999). An experimental investigation of the role of iconic gestures in lexical access using the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon. British Journal of Psychology, 90 (1), 35–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beattie, G. & Shovelton, H. (2000). Iconic hand gestures and the predictability of words in context in spontaneous speech. British Journal of Psychology, 91 (4), 473–491. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bello, A., Capirci, O., & Volterra, V. (2004). Lexical production in children with Williams syndrome: Spontaneous use of gesture in a naming task. Neuropsychologia, 42 (2), 201–213. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12 (1), 3–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bohnacker, U., Lindgren, J., & Öztekin, B. (2021). Storytelling in bilingual Turkish-Swedish children: Effects of language, age and exposure on narrative macrostructure. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 12 (4), 413–445. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown-Schmidt, S. & Tanenhaus, M. (2006). Watching the eyes when talking about size: An investigation of message formulation and utterance planning. Journal of Memory and Language, 54 1, 592–609. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, B. & Hadar, U. (1989). Gesture, speech, and computational stages: A reply to McNeill. Psychological Review, 96 1, 168–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colletta, J. M., Guidetti, M., Capirci, O., Cristilli, C., Demir, O. E., Kunene-Nicolas, R. N., & Levine, S. (2015). Effects of age and language on co-speech gesture production: an investigation of French, American, and Italian children’s narratives. Journal of Child Language, 42 (1), 122–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colletta, J. M., Pellenq, C., & Guidetti, M. (2010). Age-related changes in co-speech gesture and narrative: Evidence from French children and adults. Speech Communication, 52 (6), 565–576. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cook, S. W. & Fenn, K. M. (2017). The function of gesture in learning and memory. In R. B. Church, M. W. Alibali, & S. D. Kelly (Eds.), Why gesture?: How the hands function in speaking, thinking and communicating (pp. 129–153). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daley, L., Gokhman, E., Pechey, C., & Nicoladis, E. (in press). Predicting gesture use among bilingual and monolingual children. In A. Brown and S. Eskildsen (Eds.), Multimodality across Epistemologies in Second Language Research. London: Routledge.
Degani, T., Kreiser, V., & Novogrodsky, R. (2019). The joint effects of bilingualism, DLD and item frequency on children’s lexical-retrieval performance. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 54 (3), 485–498. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M. & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Examiner’s manual for the PPVT–III: Peabody picture vocabulary test–third Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Feyereisen, P. (2006). How could gesture facilitate lexical access?. Advances in Speech Language Pathology, 8 (2), 128–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fraundorf, S. H. & Watson, D. G. (2014). Alice’s adventures in um-derland: Psycholinguistic sources of variation in disfluency production. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29 1, 1083–1096. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frick-Horbury, D., & Guttentag, R. E. (1998). The effects of restricting hand gesture production on lexical retrieval and free recall. The American Journal of Psychology, 111 (1), 43–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58 (3), 787–814. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ha, J. W. & Sim, H. S. (2011). The relationship between lexical retrieval and coverbal gestures. Korean Journal of Cognitive Science, 22 (2), 123–143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hadar, U., Dar, R., & Teitelman, A. (2001). Gesture during speech in first and second language: Implications for lexical retrieval. Gesture, 1 (2), 151–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoetjes, M., Krahmer, E., & Swerts, M. (2015). On what happens in gesture when communication is unsuccessful. Speech Communication, 72 1, 160–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holler, J., Turner, K., & Varcianna, T. (2013). It’s on the tip of my fingers: Co-speech gestures during lexical retrieval in different social contexts. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28 (10), 1509–1518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hostetter, A. B. & Alibali, M. W. (2019). Gesture as simulated action: Revisiting the framework. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26 1, 721–752. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ivanova, I. & Costa, A. (2008). Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production?. Acta Psychologica, 127 (2), 277–288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kastenbaum, J. G., Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., Sheng, L., Mavis, I., Sebastian-Vaytadden, R., Rangamani, G., Vallila-Rohter, S., & Kiran, S. (2019). The influence of proficiency and language combination on bilingual lexical access. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22 (2), 300–330. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E. & Holler, J. (2009). Children’s use of gesture to resolve lexical ambiguity. Developmental Science, 12 1, 903–913. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kısa, Y. D., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Casasanto, D. (2022). Do gestures really facilitate speech production?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 151 (6), 1252–1271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M., Chen, Y., & Chawla, P. (1996). Nonverbal behavior and nonverbal communication: What do conversational hand gestures tell us? In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Volume 281 (pp. 389–450). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Krönke, K. M., Mueller, K., Friederici, A. D., & Obrig, H. (2013). Learning by doing? The effect of gestures on implicit retrieval of newly acquired words. Cortex, 49 (9), 2553–2568. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27 (3), 145–162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laurent, A. & Nicoladis, E. (2015). Gesture restriction affects French–English bilinguals’ speech only in French. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 18 (2), 340–349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, P. & Shirai, Y. (2000). The acquisition of lexical and grammatical aspect. Berlin & New York: De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project (3rd edn.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. (1990). Activation, competition, and frequency in lexical access. In G. T. M. Altmann (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives (pp. 148–172). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1985). So you think gestures are nonverbal? Psychological Review, 92 (3), 350–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morsella, E. & Krauss, R. M. (2004). The role of gestures in spatial working memory and speech. The American Journal of Psychology, 117 (3), 411–424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E. & Jiang, Z. (2018). Language and cognitive predictors of lexical selection in storytelling for monolingual and sequential bilingual children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 19 (4), 413–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Nagpal, J., Marentette, P., & Hauer, B. (2018). Gesture frequency is linked to story-telling style: evidence from bilinguals. Language and Cognition, 10 (4), 641–664. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Pika, S., & Marentette, P. (2009). Do French–English bilingual children gesture more than monolingual children? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 38 (6), 573–585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E. & Wiebe, S. A. (2020). How to use a wide variety of words in telling a story with a small vocabulary: cognitive predictors of lexical selection for simultaneous bilingual children. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35 (3), 330–338. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Novack, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015). Learning from gesture: How our hands change our minds. Educational Psychology Review, 27 (3), 405–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papesh, M. H. & Goldinger, S. D. (2012). Pupil-BLAH-metry: Cognitive effort in speech planning reflected by pupil dilation. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74 1, 754–765. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pika, S., Nicoladis, E., & Marentette, P. F. (2006). A cross-cultural study on the use of gestures: Evidence for cross-linguistic transfer? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9 (3), 319–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pine, K. J., Bird, H., & Kirk, E. (2007). The effects of prohibiting gestures on children’s lexical retrieval ability. Developmental Science, 10 (6), 747–754. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ping, R. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Gesturing saves cognitive resources when talking about nonpresent objects. Cognitive Science, 34 (4), 602–619. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D., Bialystok, E., Blaye, A., Polonia, A., & Yott, J. (2013). Lexical access and vocabulary development in very young bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17 (1), 57–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pyers, J. E., Magid, R., Gollan, T. H., & Emmorey, K. (2021). Gesture helps, only if you need it: Inhibiting gesture reduces Tip-of-the-Tongue resolution for those with weak Short-Term memory. Cognitive Science, 45 (1), e12914-n/a. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rauscher, F. H., Krauss, R. M. & Chen, Y. (1996). Gesture, speech and lexical access. Psychological Science, 7 (4), 226–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raymer, A., Singletary, F., Rodriguez, A., Ciampitti, M., Heilman, K., & Rothi, L. (2006). Effects of gesture verbal treatment for noun and verb retrieval in aphasia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12 1, 867–882. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rochanavibhata, S. & Marian, V. (2021). Cross-cultural differences in mother-preschooler book sharing practices in the United States and Thailand. Journal of Child Language, 48 (4), 834–857. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, S. J. & Lane, K. C. (2000). Lexical access of function versus content words. Brain and Language, 75 (3), 376–389. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Segui, J., Mehler, J., Frauenfelder, U., & Morton, J. (1982). The word frequency effect and lexical access. Neuropsychologia, 20 (6), 615–627. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smithson, L. & Nicoladis, E. (2013). Verbal memory resources predict iconic gesture use among monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16 (4), 934–944. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smithson, L., Nicoladis, E., & Marentette, P. (2011). Bilingual children’s gesture use. Gesture, 11 (3), 330–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
So, W. C. (2010). Cross-cultural transfer in gesture frequency in Chinese–English bilinguals. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25 (10), 1335–1353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stam, G. (2001). Lexical failure and gesture in second language development. In C. Cavé, I. Guaïtella, & S. Santi (Eds.), Oralité et gestualité: interactions et comportements multimodaux dans la communication (pp. 271–275). Paris: L’Harmattan.Google Scholar
Streeck, J. (2008). Depicting by gesture. Gesture, 8 (3), 285–301. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, M. D., Poarch, G. J., & Bialystok, E. (2018). Why is lexical retrieval slower for bilinguals? Evidence from picture naming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21 (3), 479–488. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Swets, B., Jacovina, M. E., & Gerrig, R. J. (2013). Effects of conversational pressures on speech planning. Discourse Processes, 50 (1), 23–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thordardottir, E. (2011). The relationship between bilingual exposure and vocabulary development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15 (4), 426–445. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Treffner, P., Peter, M., & Kleidon, M. (2008). Gestures and phases: The dynamics of speech-hand communication. Ecological Psychology, 20 (1), 32–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ünsal, Z., Jakobson, B., Wickman, P. O., & Molander, B. O. (2018). Gesticulating science: Emergent bilingual students’ use of gestures. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 55 (1), 121–144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wermelinger, S., Gampe, A., Helbling, N., & Daum, M. M. (2020). Do you understand what I want to tell you? Early sensitivity in bilinguals’ iconic gesture perception and production. Developmental Science, 23 (5), e12943. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yan, S. & Nicoladis, E. (2009). Finding le mot juste: Differences between bilingual and monolingual children’s lexical access in comprehension and production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12 (3), 323–335. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zvaigzne, M., Oshima-Takane, Y., & Hirakawa, M. (2019). How does language proficiency affect children’s iconic gesture use? Applied Psycholinguistics, 40 1, 555–583. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Tian, Mengxin
2024. An Analysis of the Efficacy of L2-MT Mixed Modeling in Second Language Acquisition - A Cognitive Ability Perspective. Applied Mathematics and Nonlinear Sciences 9:1 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 4 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.