Part of
Why Gesture?: How the hands function in speaking, thinking and communicating
Edited by Ruth Breckinridge Church, Martha W. Alibali and Spencer D. Kelly
[Gesture Studies 7] 2017
► pp. 381396
References

References

Baldwin, Dare A. and Baird, Jodie A.
2001 “Discerning intentions in dynamic human action.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5: 171–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth, Camaioni, Luigia and Volterra, Virginia
1975 “The acquisition of performatives prior to speech.” Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development 21 (3): 205–226.Google Scholar
Behne, Tanya, Liszkowski, Ulf, Carpenter, Malinda and Tomasello, Michael
2012 “Twelve-month-olds’ comprehension and production of pointing.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 30(3): 359–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beilock, Sian L. and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2010 “Gesture changes thought by grounding it in action. ” Psychological Science 21 (11): 1605–1610. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernardis, Paolo and Gentilucci, Maurizio
2006 “Speech and gesture share the same communication system.” Neuropsychologia 44 (2): 178–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bower, Gordon H. and Rinck, Mike
1999 “Goals as generators of activation in narrative understanding.” In Narrative comprehension, causality, and coherence: Essays in honor of Tom Trabasso, Susan R. Goldman, Arthur C. Graesser and Paul van den Broek (eds.), 111–134. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Broaders, Sara C., Cook, Susan W., Mitchell, Zachary and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2007 “Making children gesture brings out implicit knowledge and leads to learning.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136 (4): 539–550. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brooks, Neon and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2016 “Moving to Learn: How Guiding the Hands Can Set the Stage for Learning. ” Cognitive Science 40: 1831–1849. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capone, Nina C. and McGregor, Karla K.
2005 “The effect of semantic representation on toddlers' word retrieval.” Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 48 (6): 1468–1480. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Church, R. Breckinridge, Kelly, Spencer and Holcombe, David
2014 “Temporal synchrony between speech, action and gesture during language production.” Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29 (3): 345–354. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cook, Susan W., Duffy, Ryan G. and Fenn, Kimberly M.
2013 “Consolidation and transfer of learning after observing hand gesture.” Child development 84 (6): 1863–1871. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cook, Susan W., Mitchell, Zachary and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2008 “Gesturing makes learning last.” Cognition 106 (2): 1047–1058. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cook, Susan W. and Tanenhaus, Michael K.
2009 “Embodied communication: Speaker's gestures affect listeners' actions.” Cognition 113 (1): 98–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cook, Susan W., Yip, Terina K. and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2012 “Gestures, but not meaningless movements, lighten working memory load when explaining math.” Language and Cognitive Processes 27 (4): 594–610. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Congdon, Eliza L., Novack, Miriam A., Brooks, Neon B., Hemani-Lopez, Naureen, O’Keefe, Lucy and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2017 “Better together: Simultaneous presentation of speech and gesture in math instruction supports generalization and retention.” Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
DeLoache, Judy S.
1995 “Early understanding and use of symbols: The model model.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 4 (4): 109–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Driskell, James E. and Radtke, Paul H.
2003 “The effect of gesture on speech production and comprehension.” Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 45 (3): 445–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fausey, Caitlin M., Jayaraman, Swapnaa and Smith, Linda B.
2016 “From faces to hands: Changing visual input in the first two years.” Cognition 152: 101–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2003Hearing Gesture: How Our Hands Help Us Think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan and Beilock, Sian L.
2010 “Action’s influence on thought: The case of gesture.” Perspectives on Psychological Science 5 (6): 664–674. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan, Cook, Susan W. and Mitchell, Zachary A.
2009 “Gesturing gives children new ideas about math.” Psychological Science 20 (3): 267–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan, Nusbaum, Howard, Kelly, Spencer D. and Wagner, Susan
2001 “Explaining math: gesturing lightens the load.” Psychological Science 12 (6): 516–522. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodrich, Whitney, and Hudson Kam, Carla L.
2009 “Co-speech gesture as input in verb learning.” Developmental Science 12 (1): 81–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hilliard, Caitlin and Cook, Susan W.
2015 “A technique for continuous measurement of body movement from video.” Behavior Research Methods: 1–12.Google Scholar
Hostetter, Autumn B.
2011 “When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis.” Psychological Bulletin 137 (2): 297–315. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hostetter, Autumn B., and Alibali, Martha W.
2008 “Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 15 (3): 495–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, Jana M. and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2005 “Gesture paves the way for language development.” Psychological Science 16 (5): 368–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
James, Karin H.
2010 “Sensori-motor experience leads to changes in visual processing in the developing brain.” Developmental Science 13 (2): 279–288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
James, Karin H. and Atwood, Thea P.
2009 “The role of sensorimotor learning in the perception of letter-like forms: Tracking the causes of neural specialization for letters.” Cognitive Neuropsychology 26 (1): 91–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
James, Karin H. and Swain, Shelley N.
2011 “Only self-generated actions create sensori-motor systems in the developing brain.” Developmental science 14 (4): 673–678. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, Adam
1980 “Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance.” In The Relationship of Verbal and Nonverbal Communication, Mary R. Key (ed), 207–227. The Hague, the Netherlands: Mouton.Google Scholar
Kelly, Spencer, Healey, Meghan, Özyürek, Asli and Holler, Judith
2015 “The processing of speech, gesture, and action during language comprehension.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22 (2): 517–523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, Spencer D., Özyürek, Aslı and Maris, Eric
2010 “Two sides of the same coin speech and gesture mutually interact to enhance comprehension.” Psychological Science 21 (2): 260–267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kita, Sotaro, and Özyürek, Asli
2003 “What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking.” Journal of Memory and Language 48 (1): 16–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krehm, Madelaine, Onishi, Kristine H. and Vouloumanos, Athena
2014 “I see your point: infants under 12 months understand that pointing is communicative.” Journal of Cognition and Development 15 (4): 527–538. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
LeBarton, Eve S., Goldin-Meadow, Susan and Raudenbush, Stephen
2015 “Experimentally induced increases in early gesture lead to increases in spoken vocabulary.” Journal of Cognition and Development 16 (2): 199–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loehr, Daniel
2007 “Aspects of rhythm in gesture and speech.” Gesture 7 (2): 179–214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lucca, Kelsey R. and Wilbourn, Makeba P.
2016 “Communicating to Learn: Infants’ Pointing Gestures Facilitate Fast Mapping.” Child Development. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macedonia, Manuela, Müller, Karsten and Friederici, Angela D.
2011 “The impact of iconic gestures on foreign language word learning and its neural substrate.” Human Brain Mapping 32 (6): 982–998. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McGregor, Karla K., Rohlfing, Katharina J., Bean, Allison and Marschner, Ellen
2009 “Gesture as a support for word learning: The case of under.” Journal of Child Language 36 (4): 807–828. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeil, Nicole M., Uttal, David H., Jarvin, Linda and Sternberg, Robert J.
2009 “Should you show me the money? Concrete objects both hurt and help performance on mathematics problems.” Learning and Instruction 19 (2): 171–184. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeil, Nicole M., Alibali, Martha W. and Evans, Julia L.
2000 “The role of gesture in children's comprehension of spoken language: Now they need it, now they don't.” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 24 (2): 131–150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, David
1992Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Novack, Miriam A., Congdon, Eliza L, Hemani-Lopez, Naureen and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2014 “From action to abstraction: Using the hands to learn math.” Psychological Science, 25 (4): 903–910. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Novack, Miriam A., Goldin-Meadow, Susan and Woodward, Amanda L.
(2015) “Learning from gesture: How early does it happen?” Cognition 142: 138–147. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Novack, Miriam A., Wakefield, Elizabeth M., and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2016 “What makes a movement a gesture?” Cognition 146: 339–348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Novack, Miriam A., Wakefield, Elizabeth M., Congdon Eliza L., Franconeri, Steven and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2016 “There is more to gesture than meets the eye: Visual attention to gesture’s referents cannot account for its facilitative effects during math instruction.” In Proceedings of the 38th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Anna Papgragou, Daniel Grodner, Daneil Mirman and John Trueswell (eds.), 2141–2146. Philadelphia, PA: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Ping, Raedy M. and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2008 “Hands in the air: using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity.” Developmental Psychology 44 (5): 1277–1287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ping, Raedy and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2010 “Gesturing saves cognitive resources when talking about non-present objects.” Cognitive Science 34 (4): 602–619. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ping, Raedy M., Goldin-Meadow, Susan and Beilock, Sian L.
2014 “Understanding gesture: Is the listener’s motor system involved?” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143 (1): 195–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schachner, Adena and Carey, Susan
2013 “Reasoning about 'irrational' actions: When intentional movements cannot be explained, the movements themselves are seen as the goal.” Cognition 129 (2): 309–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Searle, John R.
1980 “The intentionality of intention and action.” Cognitive Science 4 (1): 47–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Singer, Melissa A. and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2005 “Children learn when their teacher’s gestures and speech differ.” Psychological Science 16 (2): 85–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, Tom, and Nickels, Margret
1992 “The development of goal plans of action in the narration of a picture story.” Discourse Processes 15 (3): 249–275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trofatter, Caroline, Kontra, Carly, Beilock, Sian and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2015 “Gesturing has a larger impact on problem-solving than action, even when action is accompanied by words.” Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 30 (3): 251–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Uttal, David H., Scudder, Kathyrn V. and DeLoache, Judy S.
1997 “Manipulatives as symbols: A new perspective on the use of concrete objects to teach mathematics.” Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 18 (1): 37–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valenzeno, Laura, Alibali, Martha W. and Klatzky, Roberta
2003 “Teachers’ gestures facilitate students’ learning: A lesson in symmetry.” Contemporary Educational Psychology 28 (2): 187–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wagner, Susan, Nusbaum, Howard and Goldin-Meadow, Susan
2004 “Probing the mental representation of gesture: Is handwaving spatial?” Journal of Memory and Language 50 (4): 395–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wakefield, Elizabeth M., Congdon, Eliza L., Novack, Miriam A., Goldin-Meadow, Susan and James, Karin
2017 “Learning Math by Hand: The neural effects of gesture-based instruction in 8-year-old children.” Manuscript submitted for publicationGoogle Scholar
Woodward, Amanda L.
1998 “Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach.” Cognition 69 (1): 1–34 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Woodward, Amanda L. and Guajardo, Jose J.
2002 “Infants’ understanding of the point gesture as an object-directed action.” Cognitive Development 17 (1): 1061–1084. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yoon, Jennifer M.D., Johnson, Mark H. and Csibra, Gergely
2008 “Communication-induced memory biases in preverbal infants.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105 (36): 13690–13695. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zacks, Jeffrey M., Tversky, Barbara, and Iyer, Gowri
2001 “Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130 (1): 29–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Sgard, Clara, Jean‐Christophe Bier & Philippe Peigneux
2021. Gesturing helps memory encoding in aMCI. Journal of Neuropsychology 15:3  pp. 396 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.