Part of
Perspectives on Abstract Concepts: Cognition, language and communication
Edited by Marianna Bolognesi and Gerard J. Steen
[Human Cognitive Processing 65] 2019
► pp. 241261
References
Altarriba, J., Bauer, L. M., & Benvenuto, C.
1999Concreteness, context availability, and imageability ratings and word associations for abstract, concrete, and emotion words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers 31, 578–602. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W.
2016On staying grounded and avoiding quixotic dead ends. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23, 1122–1142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, W. K., Barbey, A. K., & Wilson, C. D.
2003Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(2), 84–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W., & Wiemer-Hastings, K.
2005Situating abstract concepts. In D. Pecher, and R. A. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking (129–163). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Becker, J. A.
1994“Sneak-shoes”, “sworders”, and “nose-beards”: A case study of lexical innovation. First Language 14(41), 195–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D.
2013The acquisition of abstract words by young infants. Cognition 127, 391–397. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A.
2009Children’s acquisition of compound constructions. In R. Lieber, and P. Štekauer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of compounding (298–322). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, P.
2000How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bolognesi, M., & Steen, G.
2018Abstract Concepts: Structure, Processing and Modeling. Editors’ introduction. Topics in Cognitive Science 10(3), 490–500.Google Scholar
Booth, A. E., & Waxman, S.
2009A horse of a different color: Specifying with precision infants’ mappings of novel nouns and adjectives. Child Development 80(1), 15–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borghi, A. M., Capirci, O., Gianfreda, G., & Volterra, V.
2014The body and the fading away of abstract concepts and words: A sign language analysis. Frontiers in Psychology 5:811. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Braisby, N., Dockrell, J., & Best, R.
2001Children’s acquisition of science terms: Does fast mapping work? In M. Almgren, A. Barreña, M.-J. Ezeizabarrena, I. Idiazabal, and B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Research on child language acquisition. Proceedings of the 8th Conference of the International Association for the Study of Child Language (1066–1087). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M.
1982Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology 18(6), 906–921. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caramelli, N., Setti, A., & Maurizzi, D. D.
2004Concrete and abstract concepts in school age children. Psychology of Language and Communication 8(2), 19–34.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V.
2016First language acquisition. 3rd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Connell, L., & Lynott, D.
2011Interpretation and representation: Testing the Embodied Conceptual Combination (ECCo) Theory. In B. Kokinov, A. Karmiloff-Smith, and N. J. Nerssessian (Eds.), European perspectives on cognitive science. Proceedings of the European conference on cognitive science (paper 144). Sophia: New Bulgarian University Press.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. U., & Barbaresi, L. M
1994Morphopragmatics: Diminutives and intensifiers in Italian, German, and other languages. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Kilani-Schoch, M., & Klampfer, S.
2003How does a child detect morphology? Evidence from production. In R. H. Baayen, and R. Schreuder (Eds.), Morphological structure in language processing (391–425). Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Lettner, L. E., & Korecky-Kröll, K.
2010First language acquisition of compounds: With special emphasis on early German child language. In S. Scalise, and I. Vogel (Eds.), Cross-disciplinary issues in compounding (323–344). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elbers, L.
2000An output-as-input hypothesis in language acquisition. In P. Broeder, and J. Murre (Eds.. Models of language acquisition: Inductive and deductive approaches (244–271). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Elsen, H., & Schlipphak, K.
2015Word-formation in first language acquisition. In P. O. Müller, I. Ohnheiser, S. Olsen, and F. Rainer (Eds.), Word-formation: An international handbook of the languages of Europe, Vol. 3 (2117–2137). Berlin and Boston: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D., & Boroditsky, L.
2001Individuation, relativity and early word learning. In M. Bowerman, and S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Language, culture, and cognition Vol. 3: Language acquisition and conceptual development (215–256). New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gottfried, G.
1997Using metaphors as modifiers: Children’s production of metaphoric compounds. Journal of Child Language 24, 567–601. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jamrozik, A., McQuire, M., Cardillo, E. R., & Chatterjee, A.
2016Metaphor: Bridging embodiment to abstraction. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 23(4), 1080–1089. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kousta, S. T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E.
2011The representation of abstract words: Why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 140(1), 14–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krott, A.
2009The role of analogy for compound words. In J. Blevins, and J. Blevins (Eds.), Analogy in Grammar: Form and Acquisition (118–136). Oxford: U. Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krott, A., Gagné, C. L., & Nicoladis, E.
2010Children’s preference for HAS and LOCATED relations: A word learning bias for noun–noun compounds. Journal of Child Language 37, 373–394. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S.
1988The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development 3, 299–321. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lynott, D., & Connell, L.
2010Embodied conceptual combination. Frontiers in Psychology 1:212. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maguire, M. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M.
2006A unified theory of word learning: Putting verb acquisition in context. In K. Hirsh-Pasek, and R. M. Golinkoff (Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn verbs (364–391). New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mellenius, I
1997The acquisition of nominal compounding in Swedish. Doctoral dissertation. Lund: Lund University Press.Google Scholar
Mellenius, I.
2003Word Formation. In G. Josefsson, C. Platzack, and G. Håkansson (Eds.), The Acquisition of Swedish Grammar (75–95). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E.
2007Preschool children’s acquisition of compounds. In G. Libben, and G. Jarema (Eds.), The representation and processing of compound words (96–124). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paivio, A.
1986Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J.
1991Why are abstract concepts hard to understand? In P. J. Schwanenflugel (Ed.), The psychology of word meanings (223–250). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Shoben, E. J.
1983Differential context effects in the comprehension of abstract and concrete verbal materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 9(1), 82–102.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S.
1984Affect theory. In K. Scherer, and P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (353–400). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tribushinina, E., Voeikova, M., & Noccetti, S.
2015Adjective acquisition across languages. In E. Tribushinina, M. Voeikova, and S. Noccetti (Eds.), Semantics and morphology of early adjectives in first language acquisition (1–22). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Vigliocco. G., Kousta, S. T., Della Rosa, P. A., Vinson, D. P., Tettamanti, M., Devlin, J. T., & Cappa, S. F.
2013The neural representation of abstract words: The role of emotion. Cerebral Cortex 24(7), 1767–1777. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wauters, L., Tellings, A., Van Bon, W., & Van Haaften, A.
2003Mode of acquisition of word meanings: The viability of a theoretical construct. Applied Psycholinguistics 24(3), 385–406. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Harris, P. L., Banerjee, M., & Sinclair, A.
1995Early understanding of emotion: Evidence from natural language. Cognition and Emotion 9(2/3), 117–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wiemer-Hastings, K., Krug, J., & Xu, X.
2001Imagery, context availability, contextual constraints and abstractness. In J. D. Moore, and K. Stenning (Eds.), Proceedings of 23rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (1106–1111). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A.
2016Situation models, mental simulations, and abstract concepts in discourse comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23(4), 1028–1034. DOI logoGoogle Scholar