Chapter 8
Source–Goal asymmetry in German
A corpus study comparing intentional and non-intentional motion events
Following years of intensive research in the field of motion event representation and its encoding in language, it can be concluded that goal paths are predominantly privileged over source paths across many languages and different event types (e.g., Ikegami 1987, Lakusta and Landau 2005, Stefanowitsch and Rohde 2004, Song, this volume). This chapter has two aims: first, to cover the aspect of the granularity of lexicalization patterns encoding goal and source paths in the German language based on qualitative analysis and second, to show that in language, the factors animacy and volition are not mapped onto syntactic structures and do not directly influence the frequency of encoding goal paths. The quantitative corpus study reveals verb-specific preferences for encoding different path elements.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.The granularity of lexicalization patterns in German
- 2.1The granularity of German motion verbs
- 2.2The granularity of German adpositions
- 2.3Granularity of German complex verbs
- 3.Present study: Frequency of encoding source, route and goal paths
- 3.1Research questions
- 3.2Hypotheses and operationalization
- 3.3Design
- 3.4Results and discussion
- 4.Conclusion
-
Acknowledgements
-
References
References (30)
References
Aurnague, M. This volume. Implicit landmarks and opposite polarities in French motion predicates.
Bowerman, M. 1996. Learning how to structure space for language: A cross-linguistic perspective. In P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel & M. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space (385–436). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bowerman, M., & Choi, S. 2003. Space under construction: Language-specific categorization in first language acquisition. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind (387–427). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Garai, K., & Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I. 2002. From X to Y: The ‘complete path’ construction in Basque. Odense Working Papers in Language and Communication, 23, 289–311.
Gruber, J. S. 1965. Studies in lexical relations. Ph.D. Thesis, MIT. Cambridge: Mass.
Ikegami, Y. 1987. ‘Source’ vs. ‘goal’: A case of linguistic dissymmetry. In R. Dirven & G. Radden (Eds.), Concepts of case (122–146). Tübingen: Narr.
Jackendoff, R. 1983. Semantics and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Jackendoff, R. 1991. Parts and boundaries. Cognition, 41, 9–45.
Lakoff, G. 1987. Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lakusta, L., & Carey, S. 2014. Twelve-month-old infants’ encoding of goal and source paths in agentive and non-agentive motion events. Language Learning and Development, 11, 152–175.
Lakusta, L., & Landau, B. 2005. Starting at the end: The importance of goals in spatial language. Cognition, 96, 1–33.
Lakusta, L., Muentener, P., Petrillo, L., Mullanaphy, N., & Muniz, L. 2017. Does making something move matter? Representations of goals and sources in motion events with causal sources. Cognitive Science, 41(3), 814–826.
Lester, N. A. 2015. Linguistic input overrides conceptual biases: When goals don’t matter. Cognitive Science, 1308–1313.
Nikitina, T. 2009. Subcategorization pattern and lexical meaning of motion verbs: A study of the source/goal ambiguity. Linguistics, 47, 1113–1141.
Papafragou, A. 2010. Source-goal asymmetries in motion event representation: Implications for language production and comprehension. Cognitive Science, 34, 1064–1092.
Regier, Z., & Zheng, M. 2007. Attention to endpoints: A cross-linguistic constraint on spatial meaning. Cognitive Science, 31, 705–719.
Song, J. This volume. Source-Goal asymmetry in Standard Chinese. A comparative study of spontaneous and caused motion events.
Stathi, K. 2017. Granularity effects in event descriptions: A cross-linguistic study. Poster presented at the workshop ‘Event Representations in Brain, Language, and Development’, Max-Planck-Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, 29 October 2017.
Stefanowitsch, A., & Rohde, A. 2004. The goal bias in the encoding of motion events. In G. Radden & K.-U. Panther (Eds.), Studies in linguistic motivation (249–267). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Talmy, L. 1985. Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description (57–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Talmy, L. 1996. The windowing of attention in language. In M. Shibatani & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Grammatical constructions (235–287). New York: Clarendon Press.
Talmy, L. 2000. Toward a cognitive semantics (2 vols.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tutton, M. 2013. Granularity, space, and motion-framed location. In M. Vulchanova & E. van der Zee (Eds.), Motion encoding in language and space (149–165). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Wnuk, E. 2016. Semantic specificity of perception verbs in Maniq. Ph. D. Thesis, Radboud University Nijmegen.
Wolff, P. 2003. Direct causation in the linguistic coding and individuation of causal events. Cognition, 88, 1–48.
Zheng, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. 2002. Thought before language: How deaf and hearing children express motion events across cultures. Cognition, 85, 145–175.