Part of
Time Representations in the Perspective of Human Creativity
Edited by Anna Piata, Adriana Gordejuela and Daniel Alcaraz Carrión
[Human Cognitive Processing 75] 2022
► pp. 209232
References
Bender, A., & Beller, S.
(2014) Mapping spatial frames of reference onto time: A review of theoretical accounts and empirical findings. Cognition, 132(3), 342–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergen, B., & Chan Lau, T. T.
(2012) Writing direction affects how people map space onto time. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 109. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boltz, W. G.
(2000) The invention of writing in China. Oriens Extremus, 42, 1–17.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L.
(2001) Does language shape thought?: Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43(1), 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L., & Gaby, A.
(2010) Remembrances of times east: Absolute spatial representations of time in an Australian aboriginal community. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1635–1639. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bostan, İ., Börütecene, A., Özcan, O., & Göksun, T.
(2016) Temporal expressions in speech and gesture. In A. Papafragou, D. Grodner, D. Mirman & J. C. Trueswell (Eds.), Proceedings of The 38th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1871–1876). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Brennan, M.
(1983) Marking time in British sign language. In J. Kyle & B. Woll (Eds.), Language in sign (pp. 10–31). London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Brentari, D., Nadolske, M. A., & Wolford, G.
(2012) Can experience with co-speech gesture influence the prosody of a sign language? Sign language prosodic cues in bimodal bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(2), 402–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burns, P., McCormack, T., Jaroslawska, A. J., O’Connor, P. A., & Caruso, E. M.
(2019) Time points: A gestural study of the development of space–time mappings. Cognitive Science, 43(12), e12801. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bylund, E., & Athanasopoulos, P.
(2017) The Whorfian time warp: Representing duration through the language hourglass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(7), 911–916. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bylund, E., Gygax, P., Samuel, S., & Athanasopoulos, P.
(2020) Back to the future? The role of temporal focus for mapping time onto space. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(2), 174–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cabeza Pereiro, C., & Fernández Soneira, A.
(2004) The expression of time in Spanish Sign Language (LSE). Sign Language & Linguistics, 7(1), 63–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Callizo-Romero, C., Tutnjević, S., Pandza, M., Ouellet, M., Kranjec, A., Ilić, S., Gu, Y., Göksun, T., Chahboun, S., Casasanto, D., & Santiago, J.
(2020) Temporal focus and time spatialization across cultures. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27, 1247–1258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D.
(2016) Temporal language and temporal thinking may not go hand in hand. In B. Lewandowska Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Conceptualizations of time (pp. 169–186). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L.
(2008) Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106(2), 579–593. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Jasmin, K.
(2012) The hands of time: temporal gestures in English speakers. Cognitive Linguistics, 23(4), 643–674. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Bottini, R.
(2014) Mirror reading can reverse the flow of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2), 473–479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casey, S., & Emmorey, K.
(2009) Co-speech gesture in bimodal bilinguals. Language and Cognitive Process, 24(2), 290–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casey, S., Emmorey, K., & Larrabee, H.
(2012) The effects of learning American Sign Language on co-speech gesture. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(4), 677–686. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, J.-Y.
(2007) Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104(2), 427–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, J. Y., Friedrich, M., & Shu, H.
(2015) The effect of immediate and lifetime experience of reading horizontal and vertical texts on Chinese speakers’ temporal orientation. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 15(1–2), 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, J.-Y., & O’Seaghdha, P. G.
(2013) Do Mandarin and English speakers think about time differently? Review of existing evidence and some new data. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 41(2), 338–358.Google Scholar
Chui, K.
(2011) Conceptual metaphors in gesture. Cognitive Linguistics, 22(3), 437–458. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cienki, A.
(1998) Metaphoric gestures and some of their relations to verbal metaphorical expressions. In J. Koenig (Ed.), Discourse and cognition: bridging the gap (pp. 189–204). Stanford, CA: CSLI Productions.Google Scholar
Cooperrider, K., & Núñez, R.
de la Fuente, J., Santiago, J., Román, A., Dumitrache, C., & Casasanto, D.
(2014) When you think about it, your past is in front of you: How culture shapes spatial conceptions of time. Psychological Science, 25(9), 1682–1690. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duffy, S. E., Feist, M. I., & McCarthy, S.
(2014) Moving through time: The role of personality in three real life contexts. Cognitive Science, 38(8), 1662–1674. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duffy, S. E., & Evans, V.
(2017) The top trumps of time: Factors motivating the resolution of temporal ambiguity. Language and Cognition, 9(2), 293–315. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, K.
(2001) Space on hand: The exploitation of signing space to illustrate abstract thought. In M. Gattis (Ed.), Spatial schemas and abstract thought (pp. 147–174). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Borinstein, H. B., Thompson, R., & Gollan, T. H.
(2008) Bimodal bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 43–61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engberg-Pedersen, E.
(1999) Space and time. In J. Allwood & P. Gärdenfors (Eds.), Cognitive semantics: meaning and cognition (pp. 131–152). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fischer, S., & Gong, Q.
(2010) Variation in East Asian sign language structures. In D. Brentari (Ed.), Sign languages (pp. 502–521). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Floyd, S.
(2016) Modally hybrid grammar?: Celestial pointing for time-of-day reference in Nheengatú. Language, 92(1), 31–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuhrman, O., & Boroditsky, L.
(2010) Cross-cultural differences in mental representations of time: Evidence from an implicit nonlinguistic task. Cognitive Science, 34(8), 1430–1451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuhrman, O., McCormick, K., Chen, E., Jiang, H. D., Shu, D. F., Mao, S. M., & Boroditsky, L.
(2011) How linguistic and cultural forces shape conceptions of time: English and Mandarin time in 3D. Cognitive Science, 35, 1305–1328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P.
(2002) Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 558–565. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gu, Y., Mol, L., Hoetjes, M. W., & Swerts, M. G. J.
(2013) What can Chinese speakers’ temporal gestures reveal about their conception of time? In Proceedings of TiGeR 2013. Available at: [URL]
Gu, Y., Mol, L., Hoetjes, M., & Swerts, M.
(2017) Conceptual and lexical effects on gestures: the case of vertical spatial metaphors for time in Chinese. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 32(8), 1048–1063. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gu, Y.
(2018) Chinese hands of time. The effect of language and culture on temporal gestures and spatio-temporal reasoning. Utrecht: LOT Publisher.Google Scholar
Gu, Y., Zheng, Y., & Swerts, M.
(2019a) Which is in front of Chinese people, past or future? The effect of language and culture on temporal gestures and spatial conceptions of time. Cognitive Science, 43(12), e12804. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019b) Having a different pointing of view about the future: The effect of signs on co-speech gestures about time in Mandarin–CSL bimodal bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(4), 836–847. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gu, Y., Lin, H., & Zheng, Y. Q.
submitted). The effect of Chinese Sign Language (CSL) on CSL-Mandarin deaf bilinguals’ conceptualization of time.
Hendricks, R., & Boroditsky, L.
(2017) New space-time metaphors foster new non-linguistic representations. Topics in Cognitive Science, 9(3), 800–818. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W.
(2008) Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(3), 495–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kang, X. X., & Hu, Y. H.
(2011) Illustrated bamboo species in Shanghai (Shanghai Zhuzhong Tuzhi /上海竹种图志). Shanghai Jiao Tong University Press.Google Scholar
Kita, S., Danziger, E., & Stolz, C.
(2001) Cultural specificity of spatial schemas as manifested in spontaneous gestures. In M. Gattis (Ed.), Spatial schemas and abstract thought (pp. 115–146). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lai, V. T., & Boroditsky, L.
(2013) The immediate and chronic influence of spatiotemporal metaphors on the mental representations of time in English, Mandarin, and Mandarin-English speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–10.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M.
(1980) Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Le Guen, O., & Pool Balam, L. I.
(2012) No metaphorical timeline in gesture and cognition among Yucatec Mayas. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, H., & Cao, Y.
(2018) Karma or immortality: Can religion influence space-time mappings? Cognitive Science, 42(3), 1041–1056. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lin, H., & Gu, Y.
(2022) “Hold infinity in the palm of your hand”: A functional description of time expressions through finger configurations based on the naturalistic data of Chinese Sign Language. Language and Cognition, 14(1), 61–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
submitted). Back to the past and down to the future: Asymmetric timeline expressions in Chinese Sign Language based on the naturalistic data.
Maeder, C., & Loncke, F.
(1996) Spatial, temporal and temporo-logical notions in French Sign Language: Comparative study of deaf and hearing subjects. Sign Language Studies, 90, 38–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K., & Macrae, C. N.
(2010) Moving through time. Psychological Science, 21(2), 222–223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moore, K. E.
(2011) Ego-perspective and field-based frames of reference: Temporal meanings of front in Japanese, Wolof, and Aymara. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(3), 759–776. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nilsson, A. L.
(2016) Embodying metaphors: Signed language interpreters at work. Cognitive Linguistics, 27(1), 35–65. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R., & Cooperrider, K.
(2013) The tangle of space and time in human cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(5), 220–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R., Cooperrider, K., Doan, D., & Wassmann, J.
(2012) Contours of time: Topographic construals of past, present, and future in the Yupno valley of Papua New Guinea. Cognition, 124(1), 25–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R. E., & Sweetser, E.
(2006) With the future behind them: convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30(3), 401–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pagán-Cánovas, C., Valenzuela, J., Alcaraz-Carrión, D., Olza, I., & Ramscar, M.
(2020) Quantifying the speech-gesture relation with massive multimodal datasets: Informativity in time expressions. PLOS ONE, 15(6), e0233892. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pyers, J., & Emmorey, K.
(2008) The face of bimodal bilingualism: Grammatical markers in American Sign Language are produced when bilinguals speak to English monolinguals. Psychological Science, 19, 531–536. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez, L.
(2019) “Time is not a line”: Temporal gestures in Chol Mayan. Journal of Pragmatics, 151, 1–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saj, A., Fuhrman, O., Vuilleumier, P., & Boroditsky, L.
(2014) Patients with left spatial neglect also neglect the “Left Side” of time. Psychological Science, 25(1), 207–214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Santiago, J., Lupáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J.
(2007) Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 512–516. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schermer, T., & Koolhof, C.
(1990) The reality of time-lines: Aspects of tense in Sign Language of the Netherlands (SLN). In S. Prillwitz & T. Vollhaber (Eds.), Current trends in European sign language research (pp. 295–305). Hamburg: Signum.Google Scholar
Sinte, A.
(2013) Expression of time in French Belgian Sign Language (LSFB). In L. Meurant, A. Sinte, M. Van Herreweghe & M. Vermeerbergen (Eds.), Sign language research, uses and practices: Crossing views on theoretical and applied sign language linguistics (pp. 205–235). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I.
(1996) From ‘thought and language’ to ‘thinking for speaking.’ In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Studies in the social and cultural foundations of language, No. 17. Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 70–96). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stites, L., & Özçalışkan, Ş.
(2021) The time is at hand: Literacy predicts changes in children’s gestures about time. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 50, 967–983. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, K., & Bui, L. T.
(2016) With the future coming up behind them: Evidence that time approaches from behind in Vietnamese. Cognitive Linguistics, 27(2), 205–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tillman, K. A., Tulagan, N., Fukuda, E., & Barner, D.
(2018) The mental timeline is gradually constructed in childhood. Developmental science, 21(6), e12679. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., & Lupáñez, J.
(2006) Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cognitive Science, 30(4), 745–757. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ulrich, R., Eikmeier, V., de la Vega, I., Ruiz Fernández, S., Alex-Ruf, S., & Maienborn, C.
(2012) With the past behind and the future ahead: Back-to-front representation of past and future sentences. Memory and Cognition, 40(3), 483–495. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valenzuela, J., & Alcaraz Carrión, D.
(2020) Temporal expressions in English and Spanish: influence of typology and metaphorical construal. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 543933. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Walker, E., & Cooperrider, K.
(2016) The continuity of metaphor: Evidence from temporal gestures. Cognitive Science, 40(2), 481–495. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weisberg, J., Casey, S., Sehyr, Z. S., & Emmorey, K.
(2020) Second language acquisition of American Sign Language influences co-speech gesture production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(3), 473–482. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, S.
(2002) The iconic mapping of space and time in signed languages. In L. Albertazzi (Ed.), Unfolding perceptual continua (pp. 255–281). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wu, L., & Li, H.
(2012) Time-space metaphors in Chinese Sign Language (中国手语中的时间空间隐喻). Chinese Journal of Special Education, 150(12), 25–29.Google Scholar
Xiao, C., Zhao, M., & Chen, L.
(2018) Both earlier times and the future are “front”: the distinction between time- and ego-reference-points in mandarin speakers’ temporal representation. Cognitive Science, 42(3), 1026–1040. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Xu, D.
(2016) Vertical time expressions in ancient Chinese (古汉语里的纵向时间表达). Linguistics Science, 15(1), 1–9. [URL]
Yang, J. H.
(2015) Chinese Sign Language. In Jepsen, J. B., De Clerck, G., Lutalo-Kiingi, S. & McGregor, W. B. (Eds.), Sign languages of the world: A comparative handbook (pp. 177–194). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, W., & Sun, Y.
(2016) A monolingual mind can have two time lines: Exploring space-time mappings in Mandarin monolinguals. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(3), 857–864. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yu, N.
(2012) The metaphorical orientation of time in Chinese. Journal of Pragmatics, 44(10), 1335–1354. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zheng, X.
(2009) A study of signs for non-visual concepts in Shanghai Variety of Chinese Sign Language (上海手语非视觉概念表达研究). PhD Dissertation, Fudan University, China.
Zheng, Y. Q., Gu, Y., Cozijn, R., & Swerts, M.
(2020) Looking downwards to the future: The Chinese mind’s eye in time space. In Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (2949). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar