Chapter published in:
Multidisciplinary Approaches to Bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone World
Edited by Kate Bellamy, Michael W. Child, Paz González, Antje Muntendam and M. Carmen Parafita Couto
[Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics 13] 2017
► pp. 6792
References

References

Belletti, A., Bennati, E., & Sorace, A.
(2007) Theoretical and developmental issues in the syntax of subjects: Evidence from near-native Italian. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 25(4), 657–689. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blake, R.
(1983) Mood selection among Spanish-speaking children, ages 4 to 12. The Bilingual Review 10, 21–32.Google Scholar
Bookhamer, K.
(2013) The variable grammar of the Spanish subjunctive in second-generation bilinguals in New York City. New York, NY: City University of New York.Google Scholar
Borgonovo, C., & Prévost, P.
(2003) Knowledge of polarity subjunctive in L2 Spanish. In B. Beachley, A. Brown, & F. Conlin (Eds.), BUCLD 27: Proceedings of the 27th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 1; pp. 150–161). Somerville: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Borgonovo, C., Bruhn de Garavito, J., & Prévost, P.
(2008) Methodological issues in the L2 acquisition of a syntax/semantics phenomenon: How to assess L2 knowledge of mood in Spanish relative clauses. In J. Bruhn de Garavito & E. Valenzuela (Eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 10th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 13–24). Somerville MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Borgonovo, C., Bruhn de Garavito, J. & Prévost, P.
(2015) Mood selection in relative clauses. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(1), 33–69. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bosque, I.
(1989) Las categorías gramaticales. Relaciones y diferencias. Madrid: Síntesis.Google Scholar
Child, M.
(2017) The Typological Primacy Model and bilingual types: Transfer differences between Spanish/English bilinguals in L3 Portuguese acquisition. In K. Bellamy, M. Child, P. González, A. G. Muntendam, & M. C. Parafita Couto (Eds.), Multidisciplinary approaches to bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone world. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A.
(2012) Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax proper: Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax proper: Interrogative subject – verb inversion in heritage Spanish. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17(1), 71–96. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A., & Frank, J.
(2010) The acquisition of double que questions in Heritage and L2 Spanish. In Proceedings of the 2010 Canadian Linguistics Association Annual Conference, Montreal, Quebec, May.Google Scholar
De Prada Pérez
(2013) The Vulnerability Hypothesis. Paper presented at the Hispanic Linguistics Symposium, University of Ottawa.
De Prada Pérez, A., & Pascual y Cabo, D.
(2012) Interface heritage speech across proficiencies: Unaccusativity, focus, and subject position in Spanish. In K. Geeslin & M. Díaz-Campos (Eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 14th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 308–318). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Fábregas, A.
(2013) Differential object marking in Spanish: State of the art. Borealis. An International Journal of Hispanic Linguistics, 2, 1–80. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gallego, M., & Alonso-Marks, E.
(2014) Degrees of subjunctive vitality among monolingual speakers of Peninsular and Argentinian Spanish. Borealis – An International Journal of Hispanic Linguistics, 3(2), 95–104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
García, C.
(2011) Distinguishing two “synonyms”: A variationist analysis of quizá and quizás in six Spanish dialects. In J. Michnowicz & R. Dodsworth (Eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 5th Workshop on Spanish Sociolinguistics (pp. 103–112). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Giancaspro, D.
(2015) Heritage speakers’ variable (and systematic) production of subjunctive forms. Presentation given at workshop Lost in transmission? The role of attrition and input in heritage language development. University of Reading, October 9, 2015.Google Scholar
Iverson, M., Kempchinsky, P., & Rothman, J.
(2008) Interface vulnerability and knowledge of the subjunctive/indicative distinction with negated epistemic predicates in L2 Spanish. Eurosla Yearbook, 8(1), 135–163. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, R.
(1941 [1962]) Kindersprache, Aphasie, und allgemeine Lautgesetze. Uppsala: Almqvist & Wiksell.Google Scholar
Keating, G. D., VanPatten, B., & Jegerski, J.
(2011) Who was walking on the beach? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(2), 193–221. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kempchinsky, P.
(2009) What can the subjunctive disjoint reference effect tell us about the subjunctive? Lingua, 119(12), 1788–1810. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lantolf, J. P.
(1978) The variable constraints on mood in Puerto Rican-American Spanish. In Suñer, M. (Ed.), Contemporary studies in Romance linguistics (pp. 193–217). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
López-Ornat, S.
(1994) La adquisición de la lengua española. Madrid: Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Lynch, A.
(1999) The subjunctive in Miami Cuban Spanish: Bilingualism, contact, and language variability. Unpublished PhD Dissertation, University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
Molina, I.
(2008) The sociolinguistics of Castilian dialects. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 2008(193/194), 57–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S.
(2007) Interpreting mood distinctions in Spanish as a heritage language. In K. Potowski & R. Cameron (Eds.), Spanish in contact. Policy, social and linguistic inquiries (Impact: Studies in Language and Society 22) (pp. 23–40). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) Incomplete acquisition in Spanish heritage speakers: chronological age or interfaces vulnerability. In H. Chan, H. Jacob, & E. Kapia (Eds.), BUCLD 32: Proceedings of the 32nd annual Boston University Conference on language development (pp. 299–310). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
(2009) Knowledge of tense-aspect and mood in Spanish heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), 239–269. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Multiple interfaces and incomplete acquisition. Lingua, 121(4), 591–604. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R. & Perpiñán, S.
(2008) Knowledge of Wh-movement in Spanish L2 earners and Heritage speakers. In J. Bruhn de Garavito & E. Valenzuela (Eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 10th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 93–106). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., & Ionin, T.
(2010) Transfer effects in the interpretation of definite articles by Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 449–473. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Perpiñán, S.
(2011) Assessing differences and similarities between instructed heritage language learners and L2 learners in their knowledge of Spanish tense-aspect and mood (TAM) morphology. Heritage Language Journal, 8(1), 90–133.Google Scholar
Ocampo, F.
(1990) El subjuntivo en tres generaciones de hablantes bilingües. In J. J. Bergen (Ed.), Spanish in the United States: Sociolinguistic issues (pp. 39–48). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Pascual y Cabo, D., Lingwall, A., & Rothman, J.
(2012) Applying the Interface Hypothesis to heritage speaker acquisition: Evidence from Spanish mood. In D. Bamman, T. Magnitskaia, & C. Zaller (Eds.), BUCLD 30: Proceedings of the 30th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 437–448). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T.
(1998) The acquisition of mood selection in Spanish relative clauses. Journal of Child Language, 25, 585–604. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pires, A., & Rothman, J.
(2009) Disentangling sources of incomplete acquisition: An explanation for competence divergence across heritage grammars. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), 211–238. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M., & Kagan, O.
(2007) Heritage languages: In the ‘wild’ and in the classroom. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1(5), 368–395. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Portner, P.
(1997) The semantics of mood, complementation, and conversational force. Natural Language Semantics, 5, 167–212. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Quer, J.
(2001) Interpreting mood. Probus, 13(1), 81–111. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Twists of mood: The distribution and interpretation of indicative and subjunctive. Lingua, 119(12), 1779–1787. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rinke, E., & Flores. C.
(2014) Morphosyntactic knowledge of clitics by Portuguese heritage bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(4), 681–699. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J.
(2008) How pragmatically odd! Interface delays and pronominal subject distribution in L2 Spanish. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 1(2), 317–340. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sanchez-Naranjo, J.
(2014) Interpretation and grammar interaction in the Spanish subjunctive adjuncts. Borealis – An International Journal of Hispanic Linguistics, 3(1), 125–154. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C.
(1994) The gradual loss of mood distinctions in Los Angeles Spanish. Language Variation and Change, 6(3), 255–272. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., Filiaci, F., & Baldo, M.
(2009) Bilingual children’s sensitivity to specificity and genericity: Evidence from metalinguistic awareness. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(02), 239–257. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2000) Introduction: Syntactic optionality in non-native grammars. Second Language Research, 16(2), 93–102. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Selective optionality in language development. In L. Cornips & K. P. Corrigan (Eds.), Syntax and variation: Reconciling the biological and the social (Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 265) (pp. 55–80). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A., & Filiaci, F.
(2006) Anaphora resolution in near-native speakers of Italian. Second Language Research, 22(3), 339–368. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A., & Serratrice, L.
(2009) Internal and external interfaces in bilingual language development: Beyond structural overlap. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), 195–210. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Sorace, A.
(2006) Differentiating interfaces: L2 performance in syntax-semantics and syntax-discourse phenomena. D. Bamman, T. Magnitskaia, & C. Zaller (Eds.), BUCLD 30: Proceedings of the 30th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 653–664). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I., Sorace, A., Heycock, C., & Filiaci, F.
(2004) First language attrition and syntactic subjects: A study of Greek and Italian near-native speakers of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 8(3), 257–277. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
White, L.
(2011) Second language acquisition at the interfaces. Lingua, 121(4), 577–590. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by other publications

Child, Michael W.
2017.  In Multidisciplinary Approaches to Bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone World [Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 13],  pp. 41 ff. Crossref logo
Muysken, Pieter
2017.  In Multidisciplinary Approaches to Bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone World [Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 13],  pp. 313 ff. Crossref logo
Perez-Cortes, Silvia
2020. Lexical frequency and morphological regularity as sources of heritage speaker variability in the acquisition of mood. Second Language Research  pp. 026765832091862 ff. Crossref logo
Sainzmaza-Lecanda, Lorena & Scott A. Schwenter
2017.  In Multidisciplinary Approaches to Bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone World [Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 13],  pp. 95 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 29 october 2020. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.