Article published In:
International Journal of Chinese Linguistics
Vol. 8:1 (2021) ► pp.73101
References
Adelt, A., Stadie, N., Lassotta, R., Adani, F., & Burchert, F.
(2017) Feature dissimilarities in the processing of German relative clauses in aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 441: 17–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arabatzi, M., & Edwards, S.
(2002) Tense and syntactic processes in agrammatic speech. Brain and Language, 80(3), 314–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Awad, M., Warren, J. E., Scott, S. K., Turkheimer, F. E., & Wise, R. J. S.
(2007) A common system for the comprehension and production of narrative speech. Journal of Neuroscience, 271, 11455–11464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Badan, L., & Gobbo, F.
(2010) On the syntax of topic and focus in Chinese. In M. N. BenincàP (Ed.), Mapping the Left Periphery. The Cartography of Syntactic Structures Vol. 5 (pp. 63–90). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barbieri, E., Basso, A., Frustaci, M., & Luzzatti, C.
(2010) Argument structure deficits in aphasia: New perspective on models of lexical production. Aphasiology, 241, 1400–1423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bastiaanse, R.
(2008) Production of verbs in base position by Dutch agrammatic speakers: Inflection versus finiteness. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(2), 104–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bastiaanse, R., & Thompson, C. K.
(2003) Verb and auxiliary movement in agrammatic Broca‘s aphasia. Brain and Language 2003, 84(2), 286–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bastiaanse, R., & Van Zonneveld, R.
(2005) Sentence production with verbs of alternating transitivity in agrammatic Broca’s aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 18 (1), 57–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Benedet, M. J., Christianse, J. A., & Goodglass, H.
(1998) A cross-linguistic study of grammatical morphology in Spanish- and English-speaking agrammatic patients. Cortex, 34(3), 309–336. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berndt, R. S., & Caramazza, A.
(1980) A redefinition of the syndrome of Broca’s aphasia: Implications for neuropsychological model of language. Applied Psycholinguistic, 1(3), 225–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blom, E.
(2008) The Acquisition of Finiteness. New York: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Levelt, W. J. M.
(1994) Language production: Grammatical encoding. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), (Vol. 291) (pp. 945–984). Handbook of psycholinguistics, New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bradley, D. C., Garrett, M. E., & Zurif, E. B.
(1980) Syntactic deficits in Broca’s aphasia In Caplan, D. (Ed.), Biological studies of mental processes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burchert, F., & De Bleser, R.
(2004) Passives in agrammatic sentence comprehension: A German study. Aphasiology, 18(1), 29–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burchert, F., Swoboda-Moll, M., & Bleser, R.
(2005) Tense and agreement dissociation in German agrammatic speakers: Underspecification vs. hierarchy. Brain and Language, 94(2), 188–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D.
(1985) Syntactic and semantic structures in agrammatism. In M. L. Kean (Ed.), Agrammatism (pp. 125–152). New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, A., & Zurif, E. B.
(1976) Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 31, 572–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cinque, G.
(1999) Adverbs and Functional Heads: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cinque, G., & Rizzi, L.
(2008) The cartography of syntactic structures. Vol 21 (pp. 43–59). In V. Moscati (Ed.), CISCL Working Papers on Language and Cognition.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1970) Remarks on nominalizations. In R. Jacobs & P. Rosenbaum (Ed.), Readings in English Transformational Grammar (pp. 184–221). Waltham, MA: Ginn & co.Google Scholar
(1986) Barriers. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1991) Some notes on economy of derivation and representation. In R. Freidin (Ed.), Principles and Parameters in Comparative Grammar (pp. 417–454). Cambridge, MA: MIT.Google Scholar
(1995) The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1999) Derivation by Phase. In MIT Occasional Papers in Linguistics 18. Cambridge, MA: MITWPL.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Ali, M.
(2009) Formal features in aphasia: Tense, agreement, and mood in English agrammatism. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22(5), 436–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Bleser, R., & Kauschke, C.
(2003) Acquisition and loss of nouns and verbs: Parallel or divergent patterns? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 161, 213–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Bleser, R., & Luzzatti, C.
(1994) Morphological processing in Italian agrammatic speaker’s syntactic implementation of inflectional morphology. Brain and Language, 46(1), 21–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Villiers, J. G.
(1978) Fourteen grammatical morphemes in acquisition and aphasia. In A. Caramazza & E. Zurif (Ed.), Language Acquisition and Language Breakdown. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.Google Scholar
Dickey, M. W., Milman, L. H., & Thompson, C. K.
(2008) Judgment of functional morphology in agrammatic aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(1), 35–65. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drai, D., & Grodzinsky, Y.
(2006) The variability debate: More statistics, more linguistics. Brain and Language, 96(2), 157–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dragoy, O., & Bastiaanse, R.
(2010) Verb production and word order in Russian agrammatic speakers. Aphasiology, 241, 28–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duffield, C. J.
(2016) Frequency, activation, and the production of verb inflection in agrammatic aphasia. Aphasiology, 301, 1283–1303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D.
(2011) The brain basis of language processing: From structure to function. Physiological Reviews, 911, 1357–1392. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D., Kotz, S. A., Scott, S. K., & Obleser, J.
(2010) Disentangling syntax and intelligibility in auditory language comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 311, 448–457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D., Meyer, M., & von Cramon, D. Y.
(2000) Auditory language comprehension: An eventrelated fMRI study on the processing of syntactic and lexical information. Brain and Language, 741, 289–300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, N.
(2001) Agrammatism and the psychological reality of the syntactic tree. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(1), 71–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) Question production in agrammatism: The tree pruning hypothesis. Brain and Language, 80(2), 160–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Degrees of severity and recovery in agrammatism: Climbing up the syntactic tree. Aphasiology, 19(10/11), 1037–1051. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, N., & Grodzinsky, Y.
(1997) Tense and agreement in agrammatic production: Pruning the syntactic tree. Brain and Language, 56(3), 397–425. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gao, S. R.
(2006) Shiyuzheng [Aphasia]. Beijing: Peking University Medical Press.Google Scholar
Gavarró, A., & Dotti, H.
(2014) Long and short passives in Catalan and Spanish. Aphasiology, 28(1), 82–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gleason, J. B., Goodglass, H., Green, E., Ackerman, N., & Hyde, M. R.
(1975) The retrieval of syntax in Broca‘s aphasia. Brain and Language, 21, 451–471. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E.
(1983) The Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Grodzinsky, Y.
(1984) The syntactic characterization of agrammatism. Cognition, 16(2), 99–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1986) Language Deficits and the theory of syntax. Brain and Language, 27(1), 135–159. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995) A restrictive theory of agrammatic comprehension. Brain and Language, 50(1), 27–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1990) The Syntactic Characterization of Agrammatism. Cambridge: MIT press.Google Scholar
(2000) The neurology of syntax: Language use without Broca’s area. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(1), 1–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) A blueprint for a brain map of syntax. In Y. Grodzinsky & K. Amunts (Eds), Broca’s Region (pp. 83–107). New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grodzinsky, Y., & Santi, A.
(2008) The battle for Broca’s region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 121, 474–480. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hagiwara, H.
(1995) The breakdown of functional categories and the economy of derivation. Brain and Language, 50(1), 92–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hagiwara, H., & Caplan, D.
(1990) Syntactic comprehension in Japanese aphasics: Effects of category and thematic role order. Brain and Language, 38(1),159–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hickok, G., & Avrutin, S.
(1995) Representations, referentiality, and processing in agrammatic comprehension: Two case studies. Brain and Language, 50(1), 10–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huang, C. T., Li, Y. H., & Li, Y. F.
(2009) The Syntax of Chinese. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Humphries, C., Love, T., Swinney, D., & Hickok, G.
(2005) Response of anterior temporal cortex to syntactic and prosodic manipulations during sentence processing. Human Brain Mapping, 261, 128–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Humphries, C., Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A., & Liebenthal, E.
(2006) Syntactic and semantic modulation of neural activity during auditory sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 181, 665–679. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Time course of semantic processes during sentence comprehension: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 361, 924–932. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jonkers, R., & Bastiaanse, R.
(1998) How selective are word class deficits? Two case studies of action and object naming. Aphasiology, 121, 245–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kim, M., & Thompson, C. K.
(2000) Patterns of comprehension and production of nouns and verbs in agrammatism: Implications for lexical organization. Brain and Language, 741, 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kiss, K.
(2000) Effects of verb complexity on agrammatic aphasics’ sentence production. In R. Bastiaanse & Y. Grodzinsky (Eds.), Grammatical Disorders in Aphasia (pp. 123–151). London: Whurr Publishers.Google Scholar
Kolk, H., & Heeschen, C.
(1990) Adaptation symptoms and impairment symptoms in Broca’s aphasia. Aphasiology, 41, 221–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koukoulioti, V., & Stavrakaki, S.
(2014) Producing and inflecting verbs with different argument structure: Evidence from Greek aphasic speakers. Aphasiology, 281, 1320–1349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kratzer, A.
(1996) Severing the external argument from its verb. In J. Rooryck & L. Zaring (Eds.), Phrase Structure and the Lexicon (pp. 109–137). Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Larson, R.
(1989) On the double object construction. Linguistic Inquiry, 19(3), 335–391.Google Scholar
Law, S.-P., & Cheng, M.-E.
(2002) Production of grammatical morphemes in Cantonese aphasia. Aphasiology, 16, 693–714. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, M.
(2003) Dissociations among functional categories in Korean agrammatism. Brain and Language, 841, 170–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, J., & Man, G.
(2017) Language recovery in aphasia following implicit structural priming training: A case study. Aphasiology, 31(12), 1441–1458. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W.
(1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1993) Language use in normal speakers and its disorders. In G. Blanken, J. Dittman, H. Grimm, J. C. Marshall, & C.-W. Wallesh (Eds.), Linguistic disorders and pathologies (pp. 1–15). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
(1999) Producing spoken language: A blueprint of the speaker. In C. M. Brown, & P. Hagoort (Eds.), The Neurocognition of Language (pp. 83–122). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S.
(1999) A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 221, 1–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, B.
(2006) Chinese Final Particles and the Syntax of the Periphery (Doctoral dissertation). University of Leiden, Leiden, the Netherlands.Google Scholar
Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A.
(1981) Mandarin Chinese: A Functional Reference Grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Lin, T. H.
(2001) Light Verb Syntax and the Theory of Phrase Structure (Doctoral dissertation). University of California, Irvine, CA, US.Google Scholar
Liu, F. H. An aspectual analysis of ba
(1997) Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 6(1), 51–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lu, L.
(1994) Agrammatism in Chinese (Doctoral dissertation). Boston University, Boston, MA, US.Google Scholar
Lu, C. C., Bates, E., Li, P., Tzeng, O., Hung, D., Tsai, C. H., Lee, S. E., & Chung, Y. M.
(2000) Judgments of grammaticality in aphasia: The special case of Chinese. Aphasiology, 14(10),1021–1054. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lǚ, S. X.
(1999) Xiandai Hanyu Ba Bai Ci [Eight Hundred Words in Modern Chinese]. Beijing: The Commercial Press.Google Scholar
Marshall, J. C.
(1986) The description and interpretation of aphasic language disorder. Neuropsychologia, 241, 5–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martínez-Ferreiro, S.
(2010) The Cartography of Ibero-Romance Agrammatic Deficits. Biolinguistics, 4 (4), 324–355.Google Scholar
McAllister, T., Bachrach, A., Waters, G., Michaud, J., & Caplan, D.
(2009) Production and comprehension of unaccusatives in aphasia. Aphasiology, 23(7/8), 989–1004. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miceli, G., Mazzucchi, A., Menn, L., & Goodglass, H.
(1983) Contrasting cases of Italian agrammatic aphasia without comprehension disorder. Brain and Language, 19(1), 65–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Milman, L. H., Dickey, M. W., & Thompson, C. K.
(2008) A psychometric analysis of functional category production in English agrammatic narratives. Brain and Language, 105(1), 18–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nanousi, V., Masterson, J., Druks, J., Atkinson, M.
(2006) Interpretable vs. uninterpretable features: Evidence from six Greek-speaking agrammatic patients. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 19(3), 209–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Neuhaus, E., & Penke, M.
(2008) Production and comprehension of wh-questions in German Broca’s aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(2), 150–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Packard, J. L.
(1990) Agrammatism in Chinese: A case study. In L. Menn, K. Obler, & H. Goodglass (Eds.) Agrammatic Aphasia: Cross-Language Narrative Source Book (pp. 1191–1223). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1993) A Linguistic Investigation of Aphasic Chinese Speech. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pan, V., & Paul, W.
(2016) Why Chinese SFPs are neither optional nor disjunctors. Lingua, 1701, 23–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pollock, J. Y.
(1989) Verb movement, universal grammar and the structure of IP. Linguistic Inquiry, 20(3), 365–424. Retrevied from [URL]
Paul, W.
(2005) Low IP area and left periphery in Mandarin Chinese. Recherché Linguistiques DeVincennes, 331, 111–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Why particles are not particular: Sentence-final particles in Chinese as heads of a split CP. Studia Linguistica, 68(1), 77–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paul, W., & Whitman, J.
(2017) Topic prominence. In M. Everaert & H. van Riemsdijk (Eds), The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Syntax (2nd edition). New York: John Wiley & Sons Inc. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rizzi, L.
(1997) The fine structure of the left periphery. In L. Haegeman (Ed.), Elements of Grammar: A Handbook of Generative Syntax (pp. 281–337). Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004) Locality and left periphery. In A. Belleti (Ed.), Structures and Beyond. The Cartography of Syntactic Structures Vol. 3 (pp. 223–251). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L., & Cinque, G.
(2016) Functional categories and syntactic theory. Annual Review of Linguistics, 21, 139–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rochon, E., Saffran, E. M., Berndt, R. S., & Schwartz, M. F.
(2000) Quantitative analysis of aphasic sentence production: further development and new data. Brain and Language, 721, 193–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rogalsky, C., Almeida, D., Sprouse, J., & Hickok, G.
(2015) Sentence processing selectivity in Broca’s area: Evident for structure but not syntactic movement. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 301, 1326–1338. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rogalsky, C., & Hickok, G.
(2009) Selective attention to semantic and syntactic features modulates sentence processing networks in anterior temporal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 191, 786–796. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) The role of Broca’s area in sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 231, 1664–1680. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salis, C., & Edwards, S.
(2008) Comprehension of wh-questions and declarative sentences in agrammatic aphasia: the set partition hypothesis. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(5), 375–399. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sapolsky, D., Domoto-Reilly, K., & Dickerson, B. C.
(2014) Use of the Progressive Aphasia Severity Scale (PASS) in monitoring speech and language status in PPA. Aphasiology, 28 (8/9), 993–1003. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scott, S. K., Blank, C. C., Rosen, S., & Wise, R. J. S.
(2000) Identification of a pathway for intelligible speech in the left temporal lobe. Brain, 1231, 2400–2406. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snijders, T. M., Vosse, T., Kempen, G., Van Berkum, J. J. A., Petersson, K. M., & Hagoort, P.
(2009) Retrieval and unification of syntactic structure in sentence comprehension: An fMRI study using wordcategory ambiguity. Cerebral Cortex, 191, 1493–1503. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stavrakaki, S., Alexiadou, A., Kambanaros, M., Bostantjopolou, S., & Katsarou, Z.
(2011) The production and comprehension of verbs with alternating transitivity by patients with non-fluent aphasia. Aphasiology, 25(5), 642–668. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shlonsky, U.
(2010) The cartographic enterprise in syntax. Language and Linguistics Compass, 461, 417–429. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shyu, S.
(1995) The Syntax of Focus and Topic (Doctoral dissertation). Los Angeles: University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, US.Google Scholar
Stavrakaki, S., & Kouvava, S.
(2003) Functional categories in agrammatism: evidence from Greek. Brain and Language, 86 (1), 129–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Su, Y. C., Lee, S. E., & Chung, Y. M.
(2007) Asyntactic thematic role assignment by Mandarin aphasics: a test of the trace-deletion hypothesis and the double dependency hypothesis. Brain and Language, 101(1), 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sybesma, R.
(2007) Whether we Tense-agree overtly or not. Linguistic Inquiry, 3831, 580–587. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, C. K.
(2003) Unaccusative verb production in agrammatic aphasia: The argument structure complexity hypothesis. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16(2/3), 151–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, C. K., Bonakdarpour, B., Fix, S. F., Blumenfeld, H. D., Parrish, T. B., & Gitelman, D. R., et al.
(2007) Neural correlates of verb argument structure processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(11), 1753–1767. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, C. K., & Choy, J. J.
(2009) Pronominal resolution and gap filling in agrammatic aphasia: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 381, 255–283. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, C. K., Lange, K. L., Schneider, S. L., & Shapiro, L. P.
(1997) Agrammatic and non-brain-damaged subjects’ verb and verb argument structure production. Aphasiology, 11(4/5), 473–490. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, C. K., Tait, M. E., Ballard, K. J., & Fix, S. C.
(1999) Agrammatic aphasic subjects’ comprehension of subject and object extracted wh-questions. Brain and Language, 67(3), 169–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tissot, R. J., Mountin, G., & Lhermitte, F.
(1973) L’agrammatisme. Brussels: Dessart.Google Scholar
Tsai, W. T.
(2008) Tense anchoring in Chinese. Lingua, 118(5), 675–686. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) On the syntax-semantics correspondences of Chinese modals. Zhongguo Yuwen [Studies of the Chinese Language], 31, 208–221.Google Scholar
(2015a) On the topography of Chinese modals. In U. Shlonsky Ur (Eds), Beyond Functional Sequence (pp. 275–294). New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015b) A tale of two peripheries: Evidence from Chinese adverbials, light verbs, applicatives and object fronting. In W. T. Tsai (Ed.), The Cartography of Chinese Syntax. The Cartography of Syntactic Structures Vol. 11 (pp. 1–18). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tzeng, O. J., Chen, S., & Hung, D. L.
(1991) The classifier problem in Chinese aphasia. Brain and Language, 411, 184–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Varlokosta, S., Valeonti, N., Kakavoulia, M., Lazaridou, M., Economou, A., & Protopapas, A.
(2006) The breakdown of functional categories in Greek aphasia: Evidence from agreement, tense, and aspect. Aphasiology, 20(8), 723–743. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, H. L., & Thompson, C. K.
(2016) Assessing syntactic deficits in Chinese Broca’s aphasia using the Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentene-Chinese (NAVS-C). Aphasiology, 30(7), 815–840. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, H. Y.
(2016) A Study on Functional Category Deficits of Mandarin Speaking Agrammatics (Doctoral dissertation). Tsinghua University, Beijing, China.Google Scholar
Wang, H. Y., Liu, X. M., & Yu, C. H.
in progress). A cartographic analysis of functional category deficits in Chinese agrammatism: a longitudinal case study. Journal of Beijing International Studies University.
Wang, H.-Y., Zhong, X.-Y., & Zhai, S.-Q.
(2017) There may be tense in the Chinese language: empirical evidence from a case study of Chinese agrammatism. Journal of Beijing International Studies University, 11, 45–56.Google Scholar
Wang, H. Y., Liu, X. M., Wang, X. L., Cui, G., Xie, X. F., Zhong, X. Y., Zhai, S. Q., Dong, L. H., Sui, S. H., Xin, H., Ge, L. J., & Yu, C. H.
(2019) Functional category production and degrees of severity: findings from Chinese agrammatism. Aphasiology, 33(10), 1227–1247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wenzlaff, M., & Clahsen, H.
(2004) Tense and Agreement in German Agrammatism. Brain and Language, 89(1), 57–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yiu, E. M.-L., & Worrall, L. E.
(1996) Patterns of grammatical disruption in Cantonese aphasic subjects. Asia Pacific Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing, 11, 105–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhu, D.
(1982) Yufa Jiangyi [Lectures on Grammar]. Beijing: The Commercial Press.Google Scholar
Zurif, E. B.
(1980) Language mechanisms: A neuropsychological perspective. American Scientist, 68(3), 305–311.Google Scholar