Article published in:
International Journal of Corpus Linguistics
Vol. 19:3 (2014) ► pp. 301332
Cited by

Cited by 9 other publications

Berger, Cynthia M, Scott A Crossley & Kristopher Kyle
2019. Using Native-Speaker Psycholinguistic Norms to Predict Lexical Proficiency and Development in Second-Language Production. Applied Linguistics 40:1  pp. 22 ff. Crossref logo
CROSSLEY, SCOTT, KRISTOPHER KYLE & THOMAS SALSBURY
2016. A Usage-Based Investigation of L2 Lexical Acquisition: The Role of Input and Output. The Modern Language Journal 100:3  pp. 702 ff. Crossref logo
Crossley, Scott A. & Stephen Skalicky
2019. Examining lexical development in second language learners: An approximate replication of Salsbury, Crossley & McNamara (2011). Language Teaching 52:3  pp. 385 ff. Crossref logo
Crossley, Scott A., Stephen Skalicky, Kristopher Kyle & Katia Monteiro
2019. ABSOLUTE FREQUENCY EFFECTS IN SECOND LANGUAGE LEXICAL ACQUISITION. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 41:04  pp. 721 ff. Crossref logo
Goss, Seth J & Katsuo Tamaoka
2019. Lexical accent perception in highly-proficient L2 Japanese learners: The roles of language-specific experience and domain-general resources. Second Language Research 35:3  pp. 351 ff. Crossref logo
Hall, Joan Kelly
2020. L2 classroom interaction and its links to L2 learners’ developing L2 linguistic repertoires: A research agenda. Language Teaching  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Hashimoto, Brett J. & Jesse Egbert
2019. More Than Frequency? Exploring Predictors of Word Difficulty for Second Language Learners. Language Learning 69:4  pp. 839 ff. Crossref logo
Lindstromberg, Seth & June Eyckmans
2020. The effect of frequency on learners’ ability to recall the forms of deliberately learned L2 multiword expressions. ITL - International Journal of Applied Linguistics 171:1  pp. 2 ff. Crossref logo
Monteiro, Kátia R, Crossley, Scott A & Kyle, Kristopher
2020. In Search of New Benchmarks: Using L2 Lexical Frequency and Contextual Diversity Indices to Assess Second Language Writing. Applied Linguistics 41:2  pp. 280 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 05 december 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.

References

References

Adjemian, C.
1983“The transferability of lexical properties”. In S. Gass & L. Selinker (Eds.), Language Transfer in Language Llearning. Rowley, MA: Newberry House, 250-268.Google Scholar
Baayen, R.H., Piepenbrock, R. & Gulikers, L.
1995The CELEX Lexical Database (Release 2) [CD-ROM]. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K.
1992“The relationship of form and meaning: A cross sectional study of tense and aspect in the interlanguage of learners of English as a second language”. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13 (3), 253-278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
2000Tense and Aspect in Second Language Acquisition: Form, Meaning, and Use. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bell, H.
2003Using Frequency Lists to Assess L2 texts. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Wales Swansea, Swansea.Google Scholar
Boyd, J.K. & Goldberg, A.E.
2009“Input effects within a constructionist framework”. Modern Language Journal, 93 (3), 418-429. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
The British National Corpus
2007Distributed by Oxford University Computing Services on behalf of the BNC Consortium. Available at: http://​www​.natcorp​.ox​.ac​.uk/ (accessed October 2013).
Cobb, T.
2002Web VocabProfiler. Available at: http://​www​.er​.uqam​.ca​/nobel​/r21270​/cgi​-bin​/webfreqs​/web​_vp​.cgi (accessed October 2013).Google Scholar
Cohen, J.
1992“A power primer”. Psychological Bulletin, 112 (1), 155–159. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crossley, S.A., Salsbury, T. & McNamara, D.S.
2010“The development of polysemy and frequency use in English second language speakers”. Language Learning, 60 (3), 573-605. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crossley, S.A., Salsbury, T., McNamara, D.S. & Jarvis, S.
2011“What is lexical proficiency? Some answers from computational models of speech data”. TESOL Quarterly, 45 (1), 182-193. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crossley, S.A., Salsbury, T. & McNamara, D.S.
2012“Predicting the proficiency level of language learners using lexical indices”. Language Testing, 29 (2), 240-260. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N.
2002a“Frequency effects in language processing: A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition”. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24 (2) 143-188.Google Scholar
2002b“Reflections on frequency effects in language acquisition: A response to commentaries”. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24 (2), 297-339. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
2006a“Cognitive perspectives on SLA: The associative-cognitive CREED”. AILA Review, 19, 100-121. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
2006b“Language acquisition as rational contingency learning”. Applied Linguistics, 27 (1), 1-24. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N.C.
2012“What can we count in language, and what counts in language acquisition, cognition, and use?” In S.T. Gries & D.S. Divjak (Eds.), Frequency Effects in Language Learning and Processing (Vol. 1).Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 7-34.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. & Collins, L.
2009“Input and second language acquisition: The roles of frequency, form, and function introduction to the Special Issue”. Modern Language Journal, 93 (3), 329-335. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. & Ferreira-Junior, F.
2009“Construction learning as a function of frequency, frequency distribution and function”. Modern Language Journal, 93 (3), 370-385. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gaies, S.J.
1983“The investigation of language classroom processes”. TESOL Quarterly, 17 (2), 205-217. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gass, S. & Mackey, A.
2002“Frequency effects and second language acquisition: A complex picture?” Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 249-260. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A.E.
(2003) “Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language”. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7 (5), 219-224. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldschneider, J.M. & DeKeyser, R.
2001“Explaining the 'natural order of L2 morpheme acquisition' in English: A meta-analysis of multiple determinants”. Language Learning, 51 (1), 1–50. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, J., Dale, P., Li, P.
2008“Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary”. Journal of Child Language, 35, 515-531. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A.C., McNamara, D.S., Louwerse, M.M. & Cai, Z.
2004“Coh-Metrix: Analysis of text on cohesion and language”. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36 (2), 193-202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gries, S.T.
2008“Dispersions and adjusted frequencies in corpora”. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 13 (4), 403-437. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
2010“Dispersions and adjusted frequencies in corpora: Further explorations”. In S.T. Gries, S. Wulff & M. Davies (Eds.), Corpus Linguistic Applications: Current Studies, New Directions. Amsterdam: Rodopi, 197-212.Google Scholar
Hatch, E.
1983Psycholinguistics: A Second Language Perspective. Rowley, MA: Newbury House Publishers, Inc.Google Scholar
Izumi, S., Bigelow, M., Fujiwara, M. & Fearnow, S.
1999“Testing the output hypothesis”. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21 (3), 421-452. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Izumi, S. & Bigelow, M.
2000“Does output promote noticing and second language acquisition”. TESOL Quarterly, 34 (2), 239-278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Laufer, B.
2000“Task effect on instructed vocabulary learning: The hypothesis of involvement”. Selected papers from AILA ’99 Tokyo. Tokyo: Waseda University Press, 47-62.Google Scholar
Laufer, B. & Nation, P.
1995“Vocabulary size and use: Lexical richness in L2 written production”. Applied Linguistics, 16 (3), 307–322. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
1997“Second language acquisition and the Completion Model”. In A.M.B. de Groot & J.F. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 113-142.Google Scholar
McDonough, K. & Kim, Y.
2009“Syntactic priming, type frequency, and EFL learners’ production of Wh-questions”. Modern Language Journal, 93 (3), 386-398. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mintz, T.H., Newport, E.L. & Bever, T.G.
2002“The distributional structure of grammatical categories in speech to young children”. Cognitive Science, 26 (4), 393-424. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pica, T.
1983“Methods of morpheme quantification: Their effect on the interpretation of second language data”. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 6 (1), 69–79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P. & Ellis, N.C.
2008A Handbook of Cognitive Llinguistics and Second Language Acquisition. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Shockey, L.
2003Sound Patterns of Spoken English. Malden, MA: Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Singleton, D.
1999Vocabulary Learning in Another Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Swain, M.
1985“Communicative competence: Some roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible output in its development”. In S. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.), Input in Second Language Acquisition. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, 235-253.Google Scholar
1995“Three functions of output in second language learning”. In G. Cook & B. Seidlhofer (Eds.), Principle and Practice in Applied Linguistics: Studies in Honour of H. G. Widdowson. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 125-144.Google Scholar
Toth, P.D.
2006“Processing instruction and a role for output in second language acquisition”. Language Learning, 56 (2), 319-385. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wulff, S., Ellis, N., Römer, U., Bardovi-Harlig, K. & LeBlanc, C.
2009“The acquisition of tense– aspect: Converging evidence from corpora, cognition, and learner constructions”. Modern Language Journal, 93 (3), 354–369. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zipf, G.K.
(1935) The Psycho-biology of Language. New York: Houghton-Mifflin.Google Scholar