Part of
Iconicity: East meets West
Edited by Masako K. Hiraga, William J. Herlofsky, Kazuko Shinohara and Kimi Akita
[Iconicity in Language and Literature 14] 2015
► pp. 93108
References (65)
Ahlner, F. & Zlatev, J. 2010. Cross-modal iconicity: A cognitive semiotic approach to sound symbolism. Sign Systems Studies 38(1–4): 298–348.Google Scholar
Algoe, S.B. & Haidt, J. 2009. Witnessing excellence in action: The ‘other-praising’ emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. The Journal of Positive Psychology 4(2): 105–127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Allott, R. 1995. Sound symbolism. In Language in the Würm Glaciation, U.L. Figge (ed), 15–38. Bochum: Brockmeyer.Google Scholar
Auracher, J. 2013. Synaesthetic sound iconicity: Cross-modal integration of acoustic aspects of speech with non-acoustic aspects of meaning. Poster presentation at the 23rd annual meeting of the Society for Text and Discourse in Valencia, Spain.
Auracher, J., Albers, S., Zhai, Y., Gareeva, G. & Stavniychuk, T. 2011. P is for happiness, N is for sadness: Universals in sound iconicity to detect emotions in poetry. Discourse Processes 48(1): 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Azevedo, T.M., Volchan, E., Imbiriba, L., Rodrigues, E.C., Oliveira, J.M., Oliveira, L.F., Lutterbach, L.G. & Vargas, C.D. 2005. A freezing-like posture to pictures of mutilation. Psychophysiology 42(3): 255–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berkowitz, L. 1993. Aggression: Its Causes, Consequences, and Control. New York NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Berlin, B. 1994. Evidence for pervasive synesthetic sound symbolism in ethnozoological nomenclature. In Sound Symbolism, L. Hinton, J. Nichols & J.J. Ohala (eds), 76–103. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Bremner, A.J., Caparos, S., Davidoff, J., de Fockert, J., Linnell, K.J. & Spence, C. 2013. ‘Bouba’ and ‘Kiki’ in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape-sound matches, but different shape-taste matches to Westerners. Cognition 126(2): 165–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collier, G.L. 1996. Affective synesthesia: Extracting emotion space from simple perceptual stimuli. Motivation and Emotion 20(1): 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crisinel, A.S. & Spence, C. 2009. Implicit association between basic tastes and pitch. Neuroscience Letters 464: 39–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. 1965[1872]. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. Chicago IL: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Davis, R. 1961. The fitness of names to drawings: A cross-cultural study in Tanganyika. British Journal of Psychology 52: 259–268. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davis, M. & Whalen, P.J. 2001. The amygdala: Vigilance and emotion. Molecular Psychiatry 6: 13–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ertel, S. 1969. Psychophonetik: Untersuchungen über Lautsymbolik und Motivation. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Evans, K.K. & Treisman, A. 2010. Natural cross-modal mappings between visual and auditory features. Journal of Vision 10(1): 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallace, A. & Spence, C. 2006. Multisensory synesthetic interactions in the speeded classification of visual size. Perception and Psychophysics 68(7): 1191–1203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J.H. & Jenkins, J.J. 1966. Studies in the psychological correlates of the sound system of American English, III and IV. Word 22(1–3): 207–242.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A.G., McGhee, D.E. & Schwartz, J.K.L. 1998. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74(6): 1464–1480. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, A.G., Nosek, B.A. & Banaji, M.R. 2003. Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85(2): 197–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heise, D.R. 1970. The semantic differential and attitude research. In Attitude Measurement, G.F. Summers (ed.), 235–253. Chicago IL: Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Hinton, L., Nichols, J., & Ohala, J.J. 1994. Introduction: Sound–symbolism processes. In Sound Symbolism, L. Hinton, J. Nichols, J.J. Ohala (eds), 1–12. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
von Humboldt, W. 1836. Über die Kawi-Sprache auf der Insel Java: Nebst einer Einleitung über die Verschiedenheit des menschlichen Sprachbaus und ihren Einfluss auf die geistige Entwickelung des Menschengeschlechtes, Vol. 1. Berlin: Königliche Akademie der Wissenschaften.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. & Waugh, L.R. 1987. The Sound Shape of Language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Jespersen, O. 1933. Symbolic value of the vowel I. In Linguistica; Selected Papers in English, French, and German, 283–303. Copenhagen: Levin & Munksgaad.Google Scholar
Kantartzis, K. 2011. Children and Adults’ Understanding and Use of Sound Symbolism in Novel Words. PhD dissertation, University of Birmingham.
Klink, R.R. 2000. Creating brand names with meaning: The use of sound symbolism. Marketing Letters 11(1): 5–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Köhler, W. 1929. Gestalt Psychology. New York NY: Liveright.Google Scholar
Levenson, R.W. 1994. The search for autonomic specificity. In The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions, P. Ekman & R.J. Davidson (eds), 252–257. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. 2008. Self-conscious emotions. In Handbook of Emotions, 3rd edn, M. Lewis & J.M. Haviland-Jones (eds), 742–756. New York NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lewkowicz, D.J. & Turkewitz, G. 1980. Cross-modal equivalence in early infancy: Auditory–visual intensity matching. Developmental Psychology 16: 597–607. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lindauer, M.S. 1990. The meanings of the physiognomic stimuli taketa and maluma . Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28(1): 47–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lundholm, H. 1921. The affective tone of lines. Psychological Review 28(1): 43–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marks, L.E. 1978. The Unity of the Senses: Interrelations among the Modalities. New York NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
. 2004. Cross-modal interactions in speeded classification. In The Handbook of Multisensory Processes, G. Calvert, C. Spence & B. Stein (eds), 85–105. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Maurer, D., Pathman, T. & Mondloch, C. 2006. The shape of boubas: Sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. Developmental Science 9(3): 316–322. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McDougall, W. 1960[1908]. An Introduction to Social Psychology. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Miron, M.S. 1961. A cross-linguistic investigation of phonetic symbolism. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 62(3): 623–630. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morton, E.W. 1977. On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. American Naturalist 111: 855–869. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 1994. Sound symbolism and its role in non-human bertebrate communication. In Sound Symbolism, L. Hinton, J. Nichols & J.J. Ohala, 348–365. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Newman, S.S. 1933. Further experiments in phonetic symbolism. American Journal of Psychology 45: 53–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B., Greenwald, A. & Banaji, M. 2003. Understanding and using the implicit association test, II: Method, variables and construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85: 197–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nuckolls, J.B. 1999. The case for sound symbolism. Annual Review of Anthropology 28: 225–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Osgood, C.E. 1960. The cross-cultural generality of visual-verbal synesthetic tendencies. Behavioral Science 5(2): 146–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Osgood, C.E., Tannenbaum, P.H. & Suci, G.J. 1957. The Measurement of Meaning. Urbana IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Ozturk, O., Krehm, M. & Vouloumanos, A. 2013. Sound symbolism in infancy: Evidence for sound-shape cross-modal correspondences in 4-month-olds. Journal of Children Psychology 114(2): 173–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parise, C.V. & Spence, C. 2008. Synesthetic congruency modulates the temporal ventriloquism effect. Neuroscience Letters 442(3): 257–261. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2009. When birds of a feather flock together: Synesthetic correspondences modulate audiovisual integration in non-synesthetes. PLoS 4(5): e5664. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2012. Audiovisual crossmodal correspondences and sound symbolism: A study using the implicit association test. Experimental Brain Research 220(3–4): 319–333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plato. 1857[ca. 420 BC]. Kratylos (translated into German by Friedrich E.D. Schleiermacher). <[URL]>
Plutchik, R. 1980. Emotion: A Psychoevolutionary Synthesis. New York NY: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Poffenberger, A.T. & Barrows, B.E. 1924. The feeling value of lines. The Journal of Applied Psychology 8(2): 187–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V.S. & Hubbard, E.M. 2001. Synaesthesia – A window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8: 3–34.Google Scholar
Reay, I.E. 1994. Sound symbolism. In The Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, Vol. 8, R.E. Asher, & J.M.Y Simpson (eds), 4064–4070. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Sapir, E. 1929. A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology 12(3): 225–239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, C.E., Wright, C.I., Shin, L.M., Kagan, J., Whalen, P.J., McMullin, K.G., et al. 2003. Differential amygdalar response to novel versus newly familiar neutral faces: A functional MRI probe developed for studying inhibited temperament. Biological Psychiatry 53: 854–862. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spence, C. 2011. Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics 73(4): 971–995. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stevens, J.C. & Marks, L.E. 1965. Cross-modality matching of brightness and loudness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 54: 407–411. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. 2008. The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In Handbook of Emotions, 3rd edn, M. Lewis & J.M. Haviland-Jones (eds), 114–137. New York NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tsur, R. 2006. Size–sound symbolism revisited. Journal of Pragmatics 38: 905–924. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ultan, R. 1978. Size-sound symbolism. In Universals of Human Language, Vol. 2: Phonology, J.H. Greenberg, C.A. Ferguson & E.A. Moravcsik (eds), 525–528. Stanford CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Walker, P. & Smith, S. 1985. Stroop interference based on the multimodal correlates of haptic size and auditory pitch. Perception 14: 729–736. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weierich, M.R., Wright, C.I., Negreira, A., Dickerson, B.C. & Barrett, L.F. 2010. Novelty as a dimension in the affective brain. NeuroImage 49: 2871–2878. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Westbury, C. 2005. Implicit sound symbolism in lexical access: Evidence from an interference task. Brain and Language 93: 10–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, F.A. & Rolls, E.T. 1993. The effects of stimulus novelty and familiarity on neuronal activity in the amygdala of monkeys performing recognition memory tasks. Experimental Brain Research 9: 367–382.Google Scholar