Article published in:
Interpreting
Vol. 21:1 (2019) ► pp. 115134
References
Alexieva, B.
(1993) Aptitude tests and intertextuality in simultaneous interpreting. The Interpreters’ Newsletter 51, 8–12.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A.
(1992) Working memory. Science 255 (5044), 556–559. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2000) The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 (11), 417–423. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2003) Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders 361, 189–208. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. & Hitch, G. J.
(1974) Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, Vol. 81. New York: Academic Press, 47–89.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2006) Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 60 (1), 68–79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R. & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging 191, 290–303. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M. & Ryan, J.
(2006) Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 32 (6), 1341–1354.Google Scholar
Chincotta, D. & Underwood, G.
(1998) Simultaneous interpreters and the effect of concurrent articulation on immediate memory: A bilingual digit span study. Interpreting 3 (1), 1–20. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Christoffels, I. K., de Groot, A. M. B. & Kroll, J. F.
(2006) Memory and language skills in simultaneous interpreters: The role of expertise and language proficiency. Journal of Memory and Language 54 (3), 324–345. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J. & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition 1131, 135–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M. & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition 1061, 59–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Darò, V.
(1995) Attentional, auditory and memory indexes as prerequisites for simultaneous interpreting. In J. Tommola (Ed.), Topics in interpreting research. Turku: University of Turku Centre for Translation and Interpreting, 3–10.Google Scholar
Engle, R. W., Laughlin, J. E., Tuholski, S. W. & Conway, A. R. A.
(1999) Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128 (3), 309–331. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, M. & Posner, M. I.
(2002) Testing the efficiency and independence of Attentional Networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 141, 340–347. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A. & Lang, A.-G.
(2009) Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods 411, 1149–1160. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P. & Hewitt, J. K.
(2008) Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 137 (2), 201–225. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gile, D.
(1999) Testing the effort models’ tightrope hypothesis in simultaneous interpreting – a contribution. Hermes. Journal of Linguistics 231, 153–172.Google Scholar
(2009) Basic concepts and models for interpreter and translator training. (Rev. ed.). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hervais-Adelman, A., Moser-Mercer, B. & Golestani, N.
(2015) Brain functional plasticity associated with the emergence of expertise in extreme language control. NeuroImage 1141, 264–274. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jurado, M.-B. & Rosselli, M.
(2007) The elusive nature of executive functions: A review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology Review 17 (3), 213–233. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Keiser, W.
(1965) Definitions d’aptitude. In AIIC Colloque sur l’enseignement de l’interprétation, Paris 18–19 December, 28–37.Google Scholar
Kirchner, W. K.
(1958) Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (4), 352–358. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Klingberg, T.
(2009) The overflowing brain: Information overload and the limits of working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Köpke, B. & Nespoulous, J.-L.
(2006) Working memory performance in expert and novice interpreters. Interpreting 8 (1), 1–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liu, M., Schallert, D. L. & Carroll, P. J.
(2004) Working memory and expertise in simultaneous interpreting. Interpreting 6 (1), 19–42. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., De Sa, E. & Bialystok, E.
(2011) Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 141, 588–595. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martin-Rhee, M. M. & Bialystok, E.
(2008) The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 11 (1), 81–93. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Michael, E. B., Tokowicz, N., Degani, T. & Smith, C. J.
(2011) Individual differences in the ability to resolve translation ambiguity across languages. Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics 81, 79–97.Google Scholar
Miyake, A. & Friedman, N. P.
(2012) The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science 21 (1), 8–14. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A. & Wagner, T. D.
(2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 411, 49–100. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morales, J., Padilla, F., Gómez-Ariza, C. J. & Bajo, M. T.
(2015) Simultaneous interpretation selectively influences working memory and attentional networks. Acta Psychologica 1551, 83–91. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morris, N. & Jones, D. M.
(1990) Memory updating in working memory: The role of the central executive. British Journal of Psychology 811, 111–121. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Obler, L.
(2012) Conference interpreting as extreme language use. International Journal of Bilingualism 16 (2), 177–182. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D. & Smith, P. K.
(2002) Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging 17 (2), 299–320. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, R. D. & Monsell, S.
(1995) Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124 (2), 207–231. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rothe-Neves, R.
(2003) The influence of working memory features on some formal aspects of translation performance. In F. Alves (Ed.), Triangulating translation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 97–119. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. & Babcock, R. L.
(1991) Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Developmental Psychology 271, 763–776. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Severens, E., Van Lommel, S., Ratinckx, E. & Hartsuiker, R. J.
(2005) Timed picture naming norms for 590 pictures in Dutch. Acta Psychologica 119 (2), 159–187. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Signorelli, T. M.
(2008) Working memory in simultaneous interpreters. PhD dissertation, City University of New York.Google Scholar
Simon, J. R. & Rudell, A. P.
(1967) Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology 511, 300–304. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, M., Lammertyn, J., Verbruggen, F. & Vandierendonck, A.
(2006) Tscope: A C library for programming cognitive experiments on the MS Windows platform. Behavior Research Methods 38 (2), 280–286. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Timarová, S. & Ungoed-Thomas, H.
(2008) Admission testing for interpreting courses. The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 21, 29–46. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Timarová, S., Čeňkova, I., Meylaerts, R., Hertog, E., Szmalec, A. & Duyck, W.
(2014) Simultaneous interpreting and working memory executive control. Interpreting 16 (2), 139–168. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Turner, S. E. & Bowen, W. G.
(1999) The changing (unchanging) gender gap. Industrial and Labor Relations Review 52 (2), 289–313. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tzou, Y.-Z., Eslami, Z. R., Chen, H.-C. & Vaid, J.
(2012) Effects of language proficiency and degree of formal training in simultaneous interpreting on working memory and interpreting performance: Evidence from Mandarin-English speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism 16 (2), 213–227. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Assche, E., Duyck, W. & Hartsuiker, R. J.
(2012) Bilingual word recognition in a sentence context. Frontiers in Psychology 31, 174.Google Scholar
Verreyt, N., Woumans, E., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A. & Duyck, W.
(2015) The influence of language switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D.
(1997) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – 3rd Edition (WAIS-3®). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Woumans, E., Ceuleers, E., Van der Linden, L., Szmalec, A. & Duyck, W.
(2015) Verbal and non-verbal cognitive control in bilinguals and interpreters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 41 (5), 1579–1586.Google Scholar
Yudes, C., Macizo, P. & Bajo, T.
(2011) The influence of expertise in simultaneous interpreting on non-verbal executive processes. Frontiers in Psychology 2, 309. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 8 other publications

No author info given
2019.  In The Neurocognition of Translation and Interpreting [Benjamins Translation Library, 147], Crossref logo
Ferreira, Aline, John W. Schwieter & Julia Festman
2020. Cognitive and Neurocognitive Effects From the Unique Bilingual Experiences of Interpreters. Frontiers in Psychology 11 Crossref logo
García, Adolfo M., Edinson Muñoz & Boris Kogan
2020. Taxing the bilingual mind: Effects of simultaneous interpreting experience on verbal and executive mechanisms. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23:4  pp. 729 ff. Crossref logo
Kirsten Malmkjær, Adriana Şerban & Fransiska Louwagie
2018.  In Key Cultural Texts in Translation [Benjamins Translation Library, 140], Crossref logo
Rosiers, Alexandra, Koen Plevoets & June Eyckmans
2020. Choosing to become an interpreter. Translation, Cognition & Behavior 3:1  pp. 25 ff. Crossref logo
胡, 敏霞
2020. The Impact of Cognitive Control on the Interpreting Proficiency of Trainee Interpreters. Modern Linguistics 08:05  pp. 647 ff. Crossref logo
胡, 敏霞
2021. Review and Commentary on the Interactive Effects of Interpreting Experience and Executive Control. Modern Linguistics 09:02  pp. 283 ff. Crossref logo
胡, 敏霞
2021. The Two-Way Relationship between Interpreting Experience and Working Memory—A Review of Empirical Evidence. Modern Linguistics 09:01  pp. 187 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 15 april 2022. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.